LOGIN TO YOUR ACCOUNT

Username
Password
Remember Me
Or use your Academic/Social account:

CREATE AN ACCOUNT

Or use your Academic/Social account:

Congratulations!

You have just completed your registration at OpenAire.

Before you can login to the site, you will need to activate your account. An e-mail will be sent to you with the proper instructions.

Important!

Please note that this site is currently undergoing Beta testing.
Any new content you create is not guaranteed to be present to the final version of the site upon release.

Thank you for your patience,
OpenAire Dev Team.

Close This Message

CREATE AN ACCOUNT

Name:
Username:
Password:
Verify Password:
E-mail:
Verify E-mail:
*All Fields Are Required.
Please Verify You Are Human:
fbtwitterlinkedinvimeoflicker grey 14rssslideshare1
Bowman, Howard; Filetti, Marco; Wyble, Brad; Olivers, Christian (2013)
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Languages: English
Types: Article
Subjects: QA, QA75
A cornerstone of the target article is that, in a predictive coding framework, attention can be modelled by weighting prediction error with a measure of precision. We argue that this is not a complete explanation, especially in the light of ERP (event-related potentials) data showing large evoked responses for frequently presented target stimuli, which thus are predicted.
  • The results below are discovered through our pilot algorithms. Let us know how we are doing!

    • Abelson, R. P. (1981) Psychological status of the script concept. American Psychologist 36(7):715-29. [JBH]
    • Adams, F. & Aizawa, K. (2001) The bounds of cognition. Philosophical Psychology 14(1):43-64. [aAC]
    • Alais, D. & Blake, R., eds. (2005) Binocular rivalry. MIT Press. [aAC]
    • Alais, D. & Burr, D. (2004) The ventriloquist effect results from near-optimal bimodal integration. Current Biology 14:257-62. [aAC]
    • Alink, A., Schwiedrzik, C. M., Kohler, A., Singer, W. & Muckli, L. (2010) Stimulus predictability reduces responses in primary visual cortex. Journal of Neuroscience 30:2960-66. [aAC, TE, LM]
    • Anderson, C. H. & Van Essen, D. C. (1994) Neurobiological computational systems. In: Computational intelligence: Imitating life, ed. J. M. Zurada, R. J. Marks & C. J. Robinson, pp. 213-22. IEEE Press. [aAC]
    • Anderson, M. L. (2006) Cognitive epistemic openness. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences 5(2):125-54. [MLA]
    • Anderson, M. L. (2007) The massive redeployment hypothesis and the functional topography of the brain. Philosophical Psychology 20(2):143-74. [aAC]
    • Angelucci, A., Levitt, J. B., Walton, E. J. S., Hupe, J. M., Bullier, J. & Lund, J. S. (2002) Circuits for local and global signal integration in primary visual cortex. Journal of Neuroscience 22:8633-864. [LM]
    • Anton-Erxleben, K., Henrich, C. & Treue, S. (2007) Attention changes perceived size of moving visual patterns. Journal of Vision 7(11):1-9. [NB]
    • Arnold, J. E., Hudson, C. L. & Tanenhaus, M. K. (2007) If you say thee uh you are describing something hard: The on-line attribution of disfluency during reference comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 33:914-30. [TAF]
    • Arthur, B. (1994) Increasing returns and path dependence in the economy. University of Michigan Press. [aAC]
    • Ashby, W. R. (1940) Adaptiveness and equilibrium. The British Journal of Psychiatry 86:478-83. [TF]
    • Ashby, W. R. (1947) Principles of the self-organizing dynamic system. Journal of General Psychology 37:125-28. [KF]
    • Ay, N., Bertschinger, N., Der, R., Güttler, F. & Olbrich, E. (2008) Predictive information and explorative behavior of autonomous robots. The European Physical Journal B - Condensed Matter and Complex Systems 63(3):32939. [DYL]
    • Baerger, D. & McAdams, D. (1999) Life story coherence and its relation to psychological well-being. Narrative Inquiry 9:69-96. [JBH]
    • Bar, M. (2007) The proactive brain: Using analogies and associations to generate predictions. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 11(7):280-89. [aAC]
    • Barlow, H. B. (1961) Possible principles underlying the transformations of sensory messages. In: Sensory communication, ed. W. Rosenblith, pp. 217-34. (Chapter 13). MIT Press. [KF, PK, TT]
    • Barrett, L. F. (2009) The future of psychology: Connecting mind to brain. Perspectives in Psychological Science 4:326-39. [aAC]
    • Barrett, L. F. & Bar, M. (2009) See it with feeling: Affective predictions during object perception. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences 364(1521):1325-34. [arAC, AKS]
    • Baugh, L. A., Kao, M., Johansson, R. S. & Flanagan, J. R. (2012) Material evidence: Interaction of well-learned priors and sensorimotor memory when lifting objects. Journal of Neurophysiology 108(5):1262-69. doi:10.1152/ jn.00263.2012. [GB]
    • Bechtel, W. (2006) Reducing psychology while maintaining its autonomy via mechanistic explanation. In: The matter of the mind: Philosophical essays on psychology, neuroscience and reduction, ed. M. Schouten & H. L. de Jong, Ch. 8. Blackwell. [MWS]
    • Beer, R. D. (2000) Dynamical approaches to cognitive science. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 4(3):91-99. [TF]
    • Bengio, Y. (2009) Learning deep architectures for AI. Foundations and Trends in Machine Learning 2(1):1-127. [rAC]
    • Berkes, P. & Wiskott, L. (2005) Slow feature analysis yields a rich repertoire of complex cell properties. Journal of Vision 5(6):579-602. [PK]
    • Berlyne, D. E. (1970) Novelty, complexity and hedonic value. Perception and Psychophysics 8:279-86. [RSS]
    • Berniker, M. & Körding, K. P. (2008) Estimating the sources of motor errors for adaptation and generalization Nature Neuroscience 11:1454-61. [aAC]
    • Betsch, B. Y., Einhäuser, W., Körding, K. P. & König, P. (2004) The world from a cat's perspective - statistics of natural videos. Biological Cybernetics 90:41-50. [PK]
    • Bindra, D. (1959) Stimulus change, reactions to novelty, and response decrement. Psychological Review 66:96-103. [aAC]
    • Blake, R. (2001) A primer on binocular rivalry, including current controversies. Brain and Mind 2:5-38. [MLA]
    • Blakemore, S., Oakley, D. & Frith, C. D. (2003) Delusions of alien control in the normal brain. Neuropsychologia 41(8):1058-67. [PG]
    • Born, R. T., Tsui, J. M. & Pack, C. C. (2009) Temporal dynamics of motion integration, In: Dynamics of visual motion processing, ed. U. Ilg & G. Masson. pp. 37-54. Springer. [aAC]
    • Bourdieu, P. (1977) Outline of a theory of practice, trans. R. Nice. Cambridge University Press. [AR]
    • Brainard, D. (2009) Bayesian approaches to color vision. In: The visual neurosciences, 4th edition, ed. M. Gazzaniga, pp. 395-408. MIT Press. [aAC]
    • Brayanov, J. B. & Smith, M. A. (2010) Bayesian and “anti-Bayesian” biases in sensory integration for action and perception in the size-weight illusion. Journal of Neurophysiology 103(3):1518-31. [GB, rAC]
    • Brown, H., Friston, K. & Bestamnn, S. (2011) Active inference, attention and motor preparation. Frontiers in Psychology 2:218. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00218. [aAC]
    • Brown, M., Dilley, L. C. & Tanenhaus, M. K. (2012) Real-time expectations based on context speech rate can cause words to appear or disappear. In: Proceedings of the 34th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, ed. N. Miyake, D. Peebles, & R. P. Cooper, pp. 1374-79. Cognitive Science Society. [TAF]
    • Brown, M., Salverda, A. P., Dilley, L. C. & Tanenhaus, M. K. (2011) Expectations from preceding prosody influence segmentation in online sentence processing. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 18:1189-96. [TAF]
    • Brown, R. G. & Hwang, P. Y. C. (1992) Introduction to random signals and applied Kalman filtering, 2nd edition. Wiley. [DRa]
    • Brown, S. (2003) Biomusicology and the three paradoxes about music. Bulletin of Psychology and the Arts 4:14-17. [LH]
    • Bruner, J. (1986) Actual minds, possible worlds. Harvard University Press. [JBH]
    • Bruner, J. (1991) The narrative construction of reality. Critical Inquiry 18(1):1- 21. [JBH]
    • Bubic, A., von Cramon, D. Y. & Schubotz, R. I. (2010) Prediction, cognition and the brain. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 4(25):1-15. [aAC]
    • Buckingham, G. & Goodale, M. A. (2010) Lifting without seeing: The role of vision in perceiving and acting upon the size weight illusion. PLoS ONE 5(3):e9709. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0009709. [GB]
    • Buhusi, C. V. & Meck, W. H. (2005) What makes us tick? Functional and neural mechanisms of interval timing. Nature Reviews: Neuroscience 6:755-65. [LH]
    • Burge, J., Fowlkes, C. & Banks, M. (2010) Natural-scene statistics predict how the figure-ground cue of convexity affects human depth perception. Journal of Neuroscience 30(21):7269-80. [aAC]
    • Burr, D., Tozzi, A. & Morrone, C. (2007) Neural mechanisms for timing visual events are spatially selective in real-world coordinates. Nature Neuroscience 10:423-25. [LH]
    • Carandini, M. (2012) From circuits to behavior: A bridge too far? Nature Neuroscience 15(4):507-509. [MWS]
    • Carandini, M., Demb, J. B., Mante, V., Tolhurst, D. J., Dan, Y., Olshausen, B. A., Gallant, J. L. & Rust, N. C. (2005) Do we know what the early visual system does? Journal of Neuroscience 25(46):10577-97. [MWS]
    • Carrasco, M. (2011) Visual attention: The past 25 years. Vision Research 51:1484- 525. [NB]
    • Carrasco, M., Ling, S. & Read, S. (2004) Attention alters appearance. Nature Neuroscience 7:308-13. [NB, rAC]
    • Chappell, J. & Sloman, A. (2007) Natural and artificial meta-configured altricial information-processing systems. International Journal of Unconventional Computing 3(3):211-39. Available at: http://www.cs.bham.ac.uk/research/projects/cosy/papers/#tr0609. [AS]
    • Chater, N. & Manning, C. (2006) Probabilistic models of language processing and acquisition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 10(7):335-44. [aAC]
    • Chemero, A. (2009) Radical embodied cognitive science. MIT Press. [MLA]
    • Chen, L. (2005) The topological approach to perceptual organization. Visual Cognition 12:553-637. [SMS]
    • Chennu, S., Craston, P., Wyble, B. & Bowman, H. (2009) Attention increases the temporal precision of conscious perception: Verifying the neural ST2 model. PLOS Computational Biology 5(11):1-13. [HB]
    • Chittka, L. & Skorupski, P. (2011) Information processing in miniature brains. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, B: Biological Sciences 278 (1707):885-88. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2010.2699. [AS]
    • Churchland, P. M. (1989) The neurocomputational perspective. MIT/Bradford Books. [arAC]
    • Churchland, P. M. (2012) Plato's camera: How the physical brain captures a landscape of abstract universals. MIT Press. [arAC]
    • Clark, A. (1987) The kludge in the machine. Mind and Language 2(4):277-300. [aAC]
    • Clark, A. (1989) Microcognition: Philosophy, cognitive science and parallel distributed processing. MIT Press/Bradford Books. [arAC]
    • Clark, A. (1993) Minimal rationalism. Mind 102(408):587-610. [rAC]
    • Clark, A. (1997) Being there: Putting brain, body and world together again. MIT Press. [MLA, aAC]
    • Clark, A. (2006a) Language, embodiment and the cognitive niche. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 10(8):370-74. [arAC]
    • Clark, A. (2006b) Material symbols. Philosophical Psychology 19(3):291-307. [AR]
    • Clark, A (2008) Supersizing the mind: Action, embodiment, and cognitive extension. Oxford University Press. [arAC]
    • Clark, A. (2012) Dreaming the whole cat: Generative models, predictive processing, and the enactivist conception of perceptual experience. Mind 121(483):753- 71. [rAC]
    • Clark, A. & Chalmers, D. (1998) The extended mind. Analysis 58(1):7-19. [aAC]
    • Clark, A. & Thornton, C. (1997) Trading spaces: Computation, representation, and the limits of uninformed learning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 20(1):57-66. [rAC]
    • Clifford, C. W. G., Webster, M. A., Stanley, G. B., Stocker, A. A., Kohn, A., Sharpee, T. O. & Schwartz, O. (2007) Visual adaptation: Neural, psychological and computational aspects. Vision Research 47:3125-31. [aAC]
    • Coltheart, M. (2007) Cognitive neuropsychiatry and delusional belief (The 33rd Sir Frederick Bartlett Lecture). The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 60(8):1041-62. [aAC]
    • Conrad, K. (1958) Die beginnende Schizophrenie. Thieme Verlag. [SMS]
    • Corlett, P. R., Frith, C. D. & Fletcher, P. C. (2009a) From drugs to deprivation: A Bayesian framework for understanding models of psychosis. Psychopharmacology (Berlin) 206(4):515-30. [aAC]
    • Corlett, P. R., Krystal, J. K., Taylor, J. R. & Fletcher, P. C. (2009b) Why do delusions persist? Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 3:12. doi: 10.3389/ neuro.09.012.2009. [aAC]
    • Corlett, P. R., Taylor, J. R., Wang, X. J., Fletcher, P. C. & Krystal, J. H. (2010) Toward a neurobiology of delusions. Progress in Neurobiology 92(3):345-69. [aAC]
    • Craig, A. D. (2003) Interoception: The sense of the physiological condition of the body. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 13(4):500-505. [AKS]
    • Craig, A. D. (2009) How do you feel - now? The anterior insula and human awareness. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 10(1):59-70. [AKS]
    • Craik, K. (1943) The nature of explanation. Cambridge University Press. [DRa]
    • Critchley, H. D. & Seth, A. K. (2012) Will studies of macaque insula reveal the neural mechanisms of self-awareness? Neuron 74(3):423-26. [AKS]
    • Critchley, H. D., Wiens, S., Rotshtein, P., Ohman, A. & Dolan, R. J. (2004) Neural systems supporting interoceptive awareness. Nature Neuroscience 7(2):189- 95. [AKS]
    • Crutchfield, J. P. & Young, K. (1989) Inferring statistical complexity. Physical Review Letters 63:105-108. [DYL]
    • Dahan, D. & Tanenhaus, M. K. (2004) Continuous mapping from sound to meaning in spoken-language comprehension: Evidence from immediate effects of verbbased constraints. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 30:498-513. [TAF]
    • Damasio, A. (2000) The feeling of what happens: Body and emotion in the making of consciousness. Harvest Books. [AKS]
    • Danckert, J., Saoud, M. & Maruff, P. (2004) Attention, motor control and motor imagery in schizophrenia: implications for the role of the parietal cortex. Schizophrenia Research 70(2-3):241-61. [PG]
    • Daprati, E., Franck, N., Georgieff, N., Proust, J., Pacherie, E., Dalery, J. & Jeanerod, M. (1997) Looking for the agent: An investigation into consciousness of action and self-consciousness in schizophreniz patients. Cognition 65:71-86. [PG]
    • Darwin, C. (1871) The descent of man and selection in relation to sex. John Murray. [PAG]
    • Davidson, D. (1974) On the very idea of a conceptual scheme. Proceedings and Addresses of the American Philosophical Association 47:5-20. [MLA]
    • Dayan, P. (1997) Recognition in hierarchical models. In: Foundations of computational mathematics, ed. F. Cucker & M. Shub, pp. 43-57. Springer. [aAC]
    • Dayan, P. & Hinton, G. (1996) Varieties of Helmholtz machine. Neural Networks 9:1385-403. [aAC]
    • Dayan, P., Hinton, G. E. & Neal, R. M. (1995) The Helmholtz machine. Neural Computation 7:889-904. [arAC, KF]
    • de Gardelle, V., Waszczuk, M., Egner, T. & Summerfield, C. (2012) Concurrent repetition enhancement and suppression responses in extrastriate visual cortex. Cerebral Cortex. [Epub ahead of print: July 18, 2012]. doi: 10.1093/cercor/ bhs211. [LM]
    • Dehaene, S. (2009) Reading in the brain. Penguin. [aAC]
    • Demos, A. P., Chaffin, R., Begosh, K. T., Daniels, J. R. & Marsh, K. L. (2012) Rocking to the beat: Effects of music and partner's movements on spontaneous interpersonal coordination. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 141:49-53. [LH]
    • Dempster, A. P., Laird, N. M. & Rubin, D. B. (1977) Maximum likelihood from incomplete data via the EM algorithm. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society, Series B 39:1-38. [aAC]
    • Deneve, S. (2008) Bayesian spiking neurons I: Inference. Neural Computation 20:91-117. [aAC]
    • Dennett, D. (1978) Brainstorms: Philosophical essays on mind and psychology. Bradford Books/MIT Press. [aAC]
    • Dennett, D. C. (1987) The intentional stance. MIT Press. [aAC]
    • Dennett, D. C. (1991) Consciousness explained. Little, Brown. [aAC]
    • Dennett, D. C. (2009) Darwin's “Strange Inversion of Reasoning”. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 106 (Suppl. 1):10061-65. [rAC, DCD]
    • den Ouden, H. E. M., Daunizeau, J., Roiser, J., Friston, K. J. & Stephan, K. E. (2010) Striatal prediction error modulates cortical coupling. Journal of Neuroscience 30:3210-19. [arAC, TE]
    • den Ouden, H. E. M, Friston, K. J., Daw, N. D., McIntosh, A. R. & Stephan, K. E. (2009) A dual role for prediction error in associative learning. Cerebral Cortex 19:1175-85. [rAC, TE]
    • Desimone, R. & Duncan, J. (1995) Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Annual Review of Neuroscience 18:193-222. [aAC]
    • de-Wit, L. H., Kubilius J., Wagemans J. & Op de Beeck, H. P. (2012) Bistable Gestalts reduce activity in the whole of V1, not just the retinotopically predicted parts. Journal of Vision 12:1-14. [LM]
    • de-Wit, L., Machilsen, B. & Putzeys, T. (2010) Predictive coding and the neural response to predictable stimuli. Journal of Neuroscience 30:8702-703. [aAC]
    • Dikker, S., Rabagliati, H., Farmer, T. A. & Pylkkanen, L. (2010) Early occipital sensitivity to syntactic category is based on form typicality. Psychological Science 21:629-34. [TAF]
    • Dilley, L. C. & McAuley, J. D. (2008) Distal prosodic context affects word segmentation and lexical processing. Journal of Memory and Language 59:294- 311. [TAF]
    • Dilley, L. C. & Pitt, M. (2010) Altering context speech rate can cause words to appear or disappear. Psychological Science 21:1664-70. [TAF]
    • Dima, D., Dietrich, D. E., Dillo, W. & Emrich, H. M. (2010) Impaired top-down processes in schizophrenia: A DCM study of ERPs. NeuroImage 52:824-32. [SMS]
    • Dima, D., Roiser, J. P., Dietrich, D. E., Bonnemann, C., Lanfermann, H., Emrich, H. M. & Dillo, W. (2009) Understanding why patients with schizophrenia do not perceive the hollow-mask illusion using dynamic causal modeling. NeuroImage 46:1180-86. [SMS]
    • Di Paolo, E. A. (2009) Extended life. Topoi 28(1):9-21. [aAC, TF]
    • Di Paolo, E. A., Rohde, M. & De Jaegher, H. (2010) Horizons for the enactive mind: Values, social interaction, and play. In: Enaction: Toward a new paradigm for cognitive science, ed. J. Stewart, O. Gapenne & E. A. Di Paolo, pp. 33-87. MIT Press. [TF]
    • Doherty, M. J., Campbell, N. M., Tsuji, H. & Phillips, W. A. (2010) The Ebbinghaus illusion deceives adults but not young children. Developmental Science 13:714- 21. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00931.x. [WAP]
    • Doherty, M. J., Tsuji, H. & Phillips, W. A. (2008) The context-sensitivity of visual size perception varies across cultures. Perception 37:1426-33. [WAP]
    • Doya, K., Ishii, S., Pouget, A. & Rao, R. eds. (2007) Bayesian brain: Probabilistic approaches to neural coding. MIT Press. [aAC]
    • Dumoulin, S. O. & Hess, R. F. (2006) Modulation of V1 activity by shape: imagestatistics or shape-based perception? Journal of Neurophysiology 95:3654- 64. [aAC]
    • Egner, T., Monti, J. M. & Summerfield, C. (2010) Expectation and surprise determine neural population responses in the ventral visual stream. Journal of Neuroscience 30(49):16601-608. [arAC, TE]
    • Einhäuser, W., Kayser, C., König, P. & Körding, K. P. (2002) Learning the invariance properties of complex cells from their responses to natural stimuli. European Journal of Neuroscience 15:475-86. [PK]
    • Einhäuser, W., Moeller, G. U., Schumann, F., Conradt, J., Vockeroth, J., Bartl, K., Schneider, E. & König, P. (2009) Eye-head coordination during free exploration in human and cat. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1164:353-66. [PK]
    • Eliades, S. J. & Wang, X. (2008) Neural substrates of vocalization feedback monitoring in primate auditory cortex. Nature 453:1102-106. [rAC, TE]
    • Eliasmith, C. (2007) How to build a brain: From function to implementation. Synthese 159(3):373-88. [aAC, NS]
    • Eliasmith, C. & Anderson, C. (2003) Neural engineering: Computation, representation, and dynamics in neurobiological systems. MIT Press. [DRa]
    • Eliasmith, C., Stewart, T. C., Choo, X., Bekolay, T., DeWolf, T., Tang, Y. & Rasmussen, D. (2012) A large-scale model of the functioning brain. Science 338 (6111):1202-205. [DRa]
    • Engel, A. K., Fries, P. & Singer, W. (2001) Dynamic predictions: Oscillations and synchrony in top-down processing. Nature Reviews: Neuroscience 2:704-16. [aAC]
    • Erlhagen, W. (2003) Internal models for visual perception. Biological Cybernetics 88:409-17. [LM]
    • Ernst, M. O. (2010) Eye movements: Illusions in slow motion. Current Biology 20 (8):R357-59. [aAC]
    • Ernst, M. O. & Banks, M. S. (2002) Humans integrate visual and haptic information in a statistically optimal fashion. Nature 415:429-33. [aAC]
    • Everitt, B., Dickinson, A. & Robbins, T. (2001) The neuropsychological basis of addictive behavior. Brain Research Reviews 36:129-38. [DRo]
    • Evrard, H. C., Forro, T. & Logothetis, N. K. (2012) Von Economo neurons in the anterior insula of the macaque monkey. Neuron 74(3):482-89. [AKS]
    • Fabre-Thorpe, M. (2011) The characteristics and limits of rapid visual categorization. Frontiers in Psychology 2:243. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00243. [aAC]
    • Farmer, T. A., Christiansen, M. H. & Monaghan, P. (2006) Phonological typicality influences on-line sentence comprehension. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 103:12203-208. [TAF]
    • Farmer, T. A., Monaghan, P., Misyak, J. B. & Christiansen, M. H. (2011) Phonological typicality influences sentence processing in predictive contexts: A reply to Staub et al. (2009) Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 37:1318-25. [TAF]
    • Feldman, H. & Friston, K. J. (2010) Attention, uncertainty, and free-energy. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 4:215. doi:10.3389/fnmuh.2010.00215. [HB, arAC, TE, WAP, MWS]
    • Feldman, J. (2010) Cognitive science should be unified: Comment on Griffiths et al. and McClelland et al. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 14(8):341. [aAC]
    • ffytche, D. H. & Howard, R. J. (1999) The perceptual consequences of visual loss: “Positive” pathologies of vision. Brain 122:1247-60. [SMS]
    • Fiorillo, C. D. (2012) Beyond Bayes: On the need for a unified and Jaynesian definition of probability and information within neuroscience. Information 3 (2):175-203. doi:10.3390/info3020175. [WAP]
    • Flanagan, J. R. & Beltzner, M. A. (2000) Independence of perceptual and sensorimotor predictions in the size-weight illusion. Nature Neuroscience 3(7):737-41. doi:10.1038/76701. [GB]
    • Flanagan, J. R., Bittner, J. P. & Johansson, R. S. (2008) Experience can change distinct size-weight priors engaged in lifting objects and judging their weights. Current Biology: CB 18(22):1742-47. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2008.09.042. [GB]
    • Fletcher, P. & Frith, C. (2009) Perceiving is believing: A Bayesian approach to explaining the positive symptoms of schizophrenia. Nature Reviews: Neuroscience 10:48-58. [aAC]
    • Földiák, P. (1990) Forming sparse representations by local anti-Hebbian learning. Biological Cybernetics 64:165-70. [TT]
    • Freeman, T. C. A., Champion, R. A. & Warren, P. A. (2010) A Bayesian model of perceived head-centred velocity during smooth pursuit eye movement. Current Biology 20:757-62. [aAC]
    • Friston K. (2002) Beyond phrenology: What can neuroimaging tell us about distributed circuitry? Annual Review of Neuroscience 25:221-50. [aAC]
    • Friston, K. (2003) Learning and inference in the brain. Neural Networks 16(9):1325- 52. [aAC]
    • Friston, K. (2005) A theory of cortical responses. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences 360(1456):815-36. [arAC, TE, MWS]
    • Friston, K. (2008) Hierarchical models in the brain. PLoS Computational Biology 4: e1000211. [TE]
    • Friston, K. (2009) The free-energy principle: A rough guide to the brain? Trends in Cognitive Sciences 13(7):293-301. [aAC, TF, AR]
    • Friston, K. J. (2010) The free-energy principle: A unified brain theory? Nature Reviews Neuroscience 11(2):127-38. [aAC, TE, KF, TF, PK, WAP, TT]
    • Friston, K. (2011a) Embodied inference: Or I think therefore I am, if I am what I think. In: The implications of embodiment (Cognition and Communication), ed. W. Tschacher & C. Bergomi, pp. 89-125. Imprint Academic. [arAC, DRo]
    • Friston, K. (2011b) What is optimal about motor control? Neuron 72:488-98. [arAC]
    • Friston, K., Adams, R. A., Perrinet, L. & Breakspear, M. (2012) Perceptions as hypotheses: Saccades as experiments. Frontiers in Psychology 3:151. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00151. [rAC]
    • Friston, K. J., Daunizeau, J. & Kiebel, S. J. (2009) Reinforcement learning or active inference? PLoS (Public Library of Science) One 4(7):e6421. [aAC]
    • Friston, K. J., Daunizeau, J., Kilner, J. & Kiebel, S. J. (2010) Action and behavior: A free-energy formulation. Biological Cybernetics 102(3):227-60. [aAC]
    • Friston, K. & Kiebel, S. (2009) Cortical circuits for perceptual inference. Neural Networks 22:1093-104. [arAC]
    • Friston, K., Mattout, J. & Kilner, J. (2011) Action understanding and active inference. Biological Cybernetics 104:137-60. [aAC]
    • Friston, K. & Stephan, K. (2007) Free energy and the brain. Synthese 159(3):417- 58. [aAC, TF, DYL]
    • Frith, C. D. (2007) Making up the mind: How the brain creates our mental world. Blackwell. [BP]
    • Frith, C. D. (2012) Explaining delusions of control: The comparator model 20 years on. Consciousness and Cognition 21(1):52-54. [AKS]
    • Frith, C. D. & Wentzer, T. S. (in press) Neural hermeneutics. In: Encyclopedia of philosophy and the social sciences, vol. 1, ed. B. Kaldis. Sage. [rAC, BP]
    • Frith, C., Perry, R. & Lumer, E. (1999) The neural correlates of conscious experience: An experimental framework. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 3(3):105. [aAC]
    • Froese, T. & Di Paolo, E. A. (2011) The enactive approach: Theoretical sketches from cell to society. Pragmatics and Cognition 19(1):1-36. [TF]
    • Froese, T. & Stewart, J. (2010) Life after Ashby: Ultrastability and the autopoietic foundations of biological individuality. Cybernetics and Human Knowing 17 (4):83-106. [TF]
    • Froese, T. & Ziemke, T. (2009) Enactive artificial intelligence: Investigating the systemic organization of life and mind. Artificial Intelligence 173(3-4):366- 500. [TF]
    • Fuster, J. M. (2001) The prefrontal cortex - an update: time is of the essence. Neuron 30:319-33. [KF]
    • Gallagher, S. (2004) Neurocognitive models of schizophrenia: a neurophenomenological critique. Psychopathology 37(1):8-19. [PG]
    • Galton, F. (1883) Inquires into human faculty and its development. MacMillan. [LH]
    • Geertz, C. (1966) Religion as a cultural system. In: The interpretation of cultures, pp. 87-125. Basic Books. [AR]
    • Geisler, W. S. & Kersten, D. (2002) Illusions, perception and Bayes. Nature Neuroscience 5(6):508-10. doi:10.1038/nn0602-508. [GB]
    • Geissler, H.-G. (1983) The inferential basis of classification: From perceptual to memory code systems. Part 1: Theory. In: Modern issues in perception, ed. H.-G. Geissler, H. Buffart, E. Leeuwenberg & V. Sarris, pp. 87-105. NorthHolland. [aAC]
    • Geissler, H.-G. (1991) Constraints of mental self-organization: The indirect validation approach toward perception. Estratto da Comunicazioni Scientifiche di Psicologia Generale 5:47-69. [aAC]
    • Geldmacher, D. S. (2003) Visuospatial dysfunction in the neurodegenerative diseases. Frontiers in Bioscience 8:e428-36. [SMS]
    • Gerrans, P. (2007) Mechanisms of madness. Evolutionary psychiatry without evolutionary psychology. Biology and Philosophy 22:35-56. [aAC]
    • Gershman, S. J. & Daw, N. D. (2012) Perception, action and utility: The tangled skein. In: Principles of brain dynamics: Global state interactions, ed. M. I. Rabinovich, K. J. Friston & P. Varona, pp. 293-312. MIT Press. [aAC, TF]
    • Gibson, J. J. (1966) The senses considered as perceptual systems. Houghton Mifflin. [AS]
    • Gibson, J. J. (1979) The ecological approach to visual perception. Houghton Mifflin. [DCD]
    • Gilbert, D. T. & Wilson, T. D. (2009) Why the brain talks to itself: Sources of error in emotional prediction. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B Biological Science 364(1521):1335-41. [AKS]
    • Glimcher, P. (2003) Decisions, uncertainty and the brain. MIT Press. [DRo]
    • Glimcher, P. (2010) Foundations of neuroeconomic analysis. Oxford University Press. [rAC, DRo]
    • Gold, J. N. & Shadlen, M. N. (2001) Neural computations that underlie decisions about sensory stimuli. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 5(10):16 238-55. [aAC]
    • Gooch, C. M., Wiener, M., Hamilton, C. A. & Coslett, B. H. (2001) Temporal discrimination of sub- and suprasecond time intervals: A voxel-based lesion mapping analysis. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience 5:1-10. [LH]
    • Gowaty, P. A. & Hubbell, S. P. (2009) Reproductive decisions under ecological constraints: It's about time. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 106:10017-24. [PAG]
    • Gowaty, P. A. & Hubbell, S. P. (2005) Chance, time allocation, and the evolution of adaptively flexible sex role behavior. Integrative and Comparative Biology 45 (5):931-44. [PAG]
    • Graesser, A. C., Millis, K. K. & Zwaan, R. A. (1997) Discourse comprehension. Annual Review of Psychology 48(1):163-89. [JBH]
    • Grahn J. A. & Brett, M. (2007) Rhythm and beat perception in motor areas of the brain. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 19(5):893-906. [RSS]
    • Grahn, J. A. & McAuley, J. D. (2009) Neural bases of individual differences in beat perception. NeuroImage 47:1894-1903. [LH]
    • Gregory, R. (1998) Brainy mind. British Medical Journal 317(7174):1693-95. [GB]
    • Gregory, R. L. (1980) Perceptions as hypotheses. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B 290(1038):181-97. [aAC, KF]
    • Griffiths, T., Chater, N., Kemp, C., Perfors, A. & Tenenbaum, J. B. (2010) Probabilistic models of cognition: Exploring representations and inductive biases. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 14(8):357-64. [aAC]
    • Griffiths, P. E. & Gray, R. D. (2001) Darwinism and developmental systems. In: Cycles of contingency: Developmental systems and evolution, eds. S. Oyama, P. E. Griffiths & R. D. Gray, pp. 195-218. MIT Press. [aAC]
    • Grill-Spector, K., Henson, R. & Martin, A. (2006) Repetition and the brain: Neural models of stimulus-specific effects. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 10(1):14-23. [aAC]
    • Grodner, D. & Sedivy, J. (2011) The effect of speaker-specific information on pragmatic inferences. In: The processing and acquisition of reference, vol. 2327, eds. E. Gibson & N. Pearlmutter, pp. 239-72. MIT Press. [TAF]
    • Grossberg, S. (2013) Adaptive Resonance Theory: How a brain learns to consciously attend, learn, and recognize a changing world. Neural Networks 37:1-47. [LM]
    • Grush, R. (2004) The emulation theory of representation: Motor control, imagery, and perception. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 27:377-442. [aAC]
    • Hajcak, G. & Foti, D. (2008) Errors are aversive. Psychological Science 19(2):103- 108. [JBH]
    • Harman, K., Humphrey K. G. & Goodale, M. A. (1999) Active manual control of object views facilitates visual recognition. Current Biology 9:1315-18. [LH]
    • Harnad, S. (1990) The symbol grounding problem. Physica D 42:335-46. [aAC]
    • Harrison, L. M., Bestmann, S., Rosa, M. J., Penny, W. & Green, G. G. R. (2011) Time scales of representation in the human brain: Weighing past information to predict future events. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 5:1-8. [LH]
    • Haugeland, J. (1998) Mind embodied and embedded. In: Having thought: Essays in the metaphysics of mind, ed. J. Haugeland, pp. 207-40. Harvard University Press. [aAC]
    • Hawkins, J. & Blakeslee, S. (2004) On intelligence. Owl Books/Times Books. [aAC, TE]
    • Hay, J. & Drager, K. (2010) Stuffed toys and speech perception. Linguistics 48:865- 92. [TAF]
    • Helbig, H. & Ernst, M. (2007) Optimal integration of shape information from vision and touch. Experimental Brain Research 179:595-605. [aAC]
    • Helmholtz, H. von (1860/1962) Handbuch der physiologischen optik, vol. 3, ed. & trans. J. P. C. Southall. Dover. (Original work published in 1860; Dover English edition in 1962). [aAC]
    • Helmholtz, H. von (1876) Handbuch der physiologischen Optik. Leopold Voss. [TE]
    • Hennig, H., Fleischmann, R., Fredebohm, A., Hagmayer, Y., Nagler, J., Witt, A., Theis, F. J. & Geisel, T. (2011) The nature and perception of fluctuations in human musical rhythms. PLoS ONE 6(10):e26457. [RSS]
    • Hesselmann, G., Kell, C. A. & Kleinschmidt, A. (2010) Predictive coding or evidence accumulation? False inference and neuronal fluctuations PloS One 5(3):9926 [LM]
    • Hesselmann, G., Sadaghiani, S., Friston, K. J. & Kleinschmidt, A. (2010) Predictive coding or evidence accumulation? False inference and neuronal fluctuations. PloS (Public Library of Science) One 5(3):e9926. [aAC]
    • Hinton, G. E. (2002) Training products of experts by minimizing contrastive divergence. Neural Computation 14(8):1711-800. [aAC]
    • Hinton, G. E. (2007a) Learning multiple layers of representation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 11:428-34. [aAC]
    • Hinton, G. E. (2007b) To recognize shapes, first learn to generate images. In: Computational neuroscience: Theoretical insights into brain function, eds. P. Cisek, T. Drew & J. Kalaska. Elsevier. [aAC]
    • Hinton, G. E. (2010) Learning to represent visual input. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, B. 365:177-84. [aAC]
    • Hinton, G. E., Dayan, P., Frey, B. J. & Neal, R. M. (1995) The wake-sleep algorithm for unsupervised neural networks. Science 268:1158-60. [aAC]
    • Hinton, G. E. & Ghahramani, Z. (1997) Generative models for discovering sparse distributed representations. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 352:1177-90. [aAC]
    • Hinton, G. E. & Nair, V. (2006) Inferring motor programs from images of handwritten digits. In: Advances in neural information processing systems 18: Proceedings of the 2005 NIPS Conference, ed. Y. Weiss, B. Scholkopf & J. Platt, pp. 515-22. MIT Press. [rAC]
    • Hinton, G. E., Osindero, S. & Teh, Y. (2006) A fast learning algorithm for deep belief nets. Neural Computation 18:1527-54. [aAC]
    • Hinton, G. E. & Salakhutdinov, R. R. (2006) Reducing the dimensionality of data with neural networks. Science 313(5786):504-507. [aAC]
    • Hinton, G. E. & van Camp, D. (1993) Keeping neural networks simple by minimizing the description length of weights. In: Proceedings of COLT-93 (Sixth Annual Conference on Computational Learning Theory, Santa Cruz, CA, July 26-28, 1993), ed. L. Pitt, pp. 5-13. ACM Digital Library. [aAC]
    • Hinton, G. E. & Zemel, R. S. (1994) Autoencoders, minimum description length and Helmholtz free energy. In: Advances in neural information processing systems 6, eds. J. Cowan, G. Tesauro & J. Alspector. Morgan Kaufmann. [aAC]
    • Hirsch, H. V. B. & Spinelli, D. (1970) Visual experience modifies distribution of horizontally and vertically oriented receptive fields in cats. Science 168:869- 71. [BB]
    • Hirsh, J. B., Mar, R. A. & Peterson, J. B. (2012) Psychological entropy: A framework for understanding uncertainty-related anxiety. Psychological Review 119 (2):304-20. [JBH]
    • Hochstein, S. & Ahissar, M. (2002) View from the top: Hierarchies and reverse hierarchies in the visual system. Neuron 36(5):791-804. [aAC]
    • Hogg, D. (1983) Model-based vision: A program to see a walking person. Image and Vision Computing 1(1):5-20. [AS]
    • Hohwy, J. (2007) Functional Integration and the mind. Synthese 159(3):315-28. [aAC]
    • Hohwy, J. (2012) Attention and conscious perception in the hypothesis testing brain. Frontiers in Psychology 3:96, 1-14. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00096. [rAC, LM]
    • Hohwy, J. & Paton, B. (2010) Explaining away the body: Experiences of supernaturally caused touch and touch on non-hand objects within the rubber hand illusion. PLoS ONE 5(2):e9416. [BP]
    • Hohwy, J., Roepstorff, A. & Friston, K. (2008) Predictive coding explains binocular rivalry: An epistemological review. Cognition 108(3):687-701. [aAC, MLA]
    • Hollensen, P. & Trappenberg, T. (2011) Learning sparse representations through learned inhibition. Poster presented at the COSYNE (Computational and Systems Neuroscience Conference) Annual Meeting, Salt Lake City, Utah, February 24, 2011. [TT]
    • Holleman, J. R. & Schultz, W. (1998) Dopamine neurons report an error in the temporal prediction of reward during learning. Nature Reviews: Neuroscience 1:304-309. [aAC]
    • Holm, L., Ullén, F. & Madison, G. (in press) Motor and executive control in repetitive timing of brief intervals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. doi 10.1037/a0029142. [LH]
    • Horstmann, G. (2002) Evidence for attentional capture by a surprising color singleton in visual search. Psychological Science 13(6):499-505. [HB]
    • Hosoya, T., Baccus, S. A. & Meister, M. (2005) Dynamic predictive coding by the retina. Nature 436(7):71-77. [aAC]
    • Howe, C. Q., Lotto, R. B. & Purves, D. (2006) Comparison of bayesian and empirical ranking approaches to visual perception. Journal of Theoretical Biology 241:866-75. [aAC]
    • Huang, Y. & Rao, R. (2011) Predictive coding. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science 2:580-93. [aAC]
    • Hubbell, S. P. & Johnson, L. K. (1987) Environmental variance in lifetime mating success, mate choice, and sexual selection. American Naturalist 130(1):91- 112. [PAG]
    • Hubel, D. H. & Wiesel, T. N. (1965) Receptive fields and functional architecture in two nonstriate visual areas (18 and 19) of the cat. Journal of Neurophysiology 28:229-89. [TE]
    • Hume, D. (1739/1888/1964) Treatise of human nature, ed. L. A. Selby-Biggs. Oxford University Press. (Original work published in 1739; OUP edition 1888; reprint 1964 source cited.) [DCD]
    • Humphrey, N. (2000) How to solve the mind-body problem. Journal of Consciousness Studies 7:5-20. [aAC]
    • Hurley, M., Dennett, D. C. & Adams, R. B., Jr. (2011) Inside jokes: Using humor to reverse-engineer the mind. MIT Press. [DCD]
    • Hurley, S. (1998) Consciousness in action. Harvard University Press. [aAC, NS]
    • Huron, D. (2006) Sweet anticipation: Music and the psychology of expectation. MIT Press. [RSS]
    • Hutchins, E. (1995) Cognition in the wild. MIT Press. [arAC]
    • Ikegami, T. (2007) Simulating active perception and mental imagery with embodied chaotic itinerancy. Journal of Consciousness Studies 14(7):111-25. [TF]
    • Iriki, A. & Taoka, M. (2012) Triadic (ecological, neural, cognitive) niche construction: A scenario of human brain evolution extrapolating tool use and language from the control of reaching actions. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 367:10-23. [aAC]
    • Jaeger, H. (2011) Neural hierarchies: Singin' the blues. Oral presentation at Osnabrück Computational Cognition Alliance Meeting (OCCAM 2011), University of Osnabrück, Germany, June 22-24, 2011. Available at: http://video. virtuos.uni-osnabrueck.de:8080/engage/ui/watch.html?id=10bc55e8-8d98- 40d3-bb11-17780b70c052&play=true. [TT]
    • Jahanshahi, M., Dirnberger, G., Fuller, R. & Frith, C. D. (2000) The role of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in random number generation: A study with positron emission tomography. NeuroImage 12:713-25. [LH]
    • James, W. (1890) The principles of psychology. Henry Holt. [AKS]
    • Janoff-Bulman, R. (1992) Shattered assumptions: Towards a new psychology of trauma. Free Press. [JBH]
    • Jaynes, E. T. (1957) Information theory and statistical mechanics. Physical Review (Series II) 106(4):620-30. [KF]
    • Jaynes, E. T. (2003) Probability theory: The logic of science. Cambridge University Press. [WAP]
    • Jeannerod, M. (2006) Motor cognition: What actions tell the self. Oxford University Press. [PG]
    • Jeannerod, M., Farrer, C.,Franck, N., Fourneret, P., Posada, A., Daprati, E. & Georgieff, N. (2003) Action recognition in normal and schizophrenic subjects. In: The self in neuroscience and psychiatry, ed. N. Kirchner & A. David, pp. 380-406. Cambridge University Press. [PG]
    • Jehee, J. F. M. & Ballard, D. H. (2009) Predictive feedback can account for biphasic responses in the lateral geniculate nucleus. PLoS (Public Library of Science) Computational Biology 5(5):e1000373. [aAC]
    • Jiang, J., Schmajuk, N. & Egner, T. (2012) Explaining neural signals in human visual cortex with an associative learning model. Behavioral Neuroscience 126(4):575- 81. [TE]
    • Johnston, A., Arnold, D. H. & Nishida, S. (2006) Spatially localized distortions of time. Current Biology 16:472-79. [LH]
    • Kalman, R. E. (1960) A new approach to linear filtering and prediction problems. Transactions of the ASME - Journal of Basic Engineering (Series D) 82:35- 45. [DRa]
    • Kant, I. (1781/1929) Critique of pure reason, trans. N. Kemp Smith. Macmillan. (Original work published in 1781; Kemp Smith translation 1929). [AS]
    • Kärcher, S. M., Fenzlaff, S., Hartmann, D., Nagel, S. K. & König, P. (2012) Sensory augmentation for the blind. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 6:37. [PK]
    • Karmarkar, U. R & Buonomano, D. V. (2007) Timing in the absence of clocks: Encoding time in neural network states. Neuron 53:427-38. [LH]
    • Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1992) Beyond modularity: A developmental perspective on cognitive science. MIT Press. [AS]
    • Kawato, M., Hayakama, H. & Inui, T. (1993) A forward-inverse optics model of reciprocal connections between visual cortical areas. Network 4:415-22. [aAC]
    • Kay, J., Floreano, D. & Phillips, W. A. (1998) Contextually guided unsupervised learning using local multivariate binary processors. Neural Networks 11:117- 40. [WAP]
    • Kay, J. & Phillips, W. A. (2010) Coherent Infomax as a computational goal for neural systems. Bulletin of Mathematical Biology 73:344-72. doi: 10.1007/s11538-010- 9564-x. [rAC, WAP]
    • Keller, G. B., Bonhoeffer, T. & Hubener, M. (2012) Sensorimotor mismatch signals in primary visual cortex of the behaving mouse. Neuron 74:809-15. [rAC, TE]
    • Khalil, E. L. (1989) Adam Smith and Albert Einstein: The aesthetic principle of truth. History of Economics Society Bulletin 11(2):222-37. [ELK]
    • Khalil, E. L. (2010) The Bayesian fallacy: Distinguishing internal motivations and religious beliefs from other beliefs. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization 75(2):268-80. doi: 10.1016/j.jebo.2010.04.004. [ELK]
    • Kinoshita, M., Gilbert, C. D. & Das, A. (2009) Optical imaging of contextual interactions in V1 of the behaving monkey. Journal of Neurophysiology 102: 1930- 44. [SMS]
    • Kitayama, S. & Cohen, D. (2010) Handbook of cultural psychology. The Guilford Press. [JBH]
    • Kleinschmidt, D. & Jaeger, T. F. (2011) A Bayesian belief updating model of phonetic recalibration and selective adaptation. Association for Computational Linguistics - Computational Modeling and Computational Linguistics. [TAF]
    • Knill, D. & Pouget, A. (2004) The Bayesian brain: The role of uncertainty in neural coding and computation. Trends in Neuroscience 27(12):712-19. [aAC]
    • Koethe, D., Kranaster, L., Hoyer, C., Gross, S., Neatby, M. A., Schultze-Lutter, F., Ruhrmann, S., Klosterkötter, J., Hellmich, M. & Leweke, F. M. (2009) Binocular depth inversion as a paradigm of reduced visual information processing in prodromal state, antipsychotic-naive and treated schizophrenia. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience 259:195-202. [SMS]
    • Kohonen, T. (1989) Self-organization and associative memory. Springer-Verlag. [aAC]
    • Kok, P., Jehee, J. F. & de Lange, F. P. (2012) Less is more: Expectation sharpens representations in the primary visual cortex. Neuron 75(2):265-70. [LM]
    • Kok, P., Rahnev, D., Jehee, J. F., Lau, H. C. & de Lange, F. P. (2011) Attention reverses the effect of prediction in silencing sensory signals. Cerebral Cortex 22:2197-206. [rAC, TE]
    • König, P. & Krüger, N. (2006) Symbols as self-emergent entities in an optimization process of feature extraction and predictions. Biological Cybernetics 94(4):325- 34. [aAC, PK]
    • Körding, K. P., Kayser, C., Einhäuser, W. & König, P. (2004) How are complex cell properties adapted to the statistics of natural stimuli? Journal of Neurophysiology 91(1):206-12. [PK]
    • Körding, K. P. & König, P. (2000) Learning with two sites of synaptic integration. Network: Computation in Neural Systems 11:1-15. [WAP]
    • Körding, K. P., Tenenbaum, J. B. & Shadmehr, R. (2007) The dynamics of memory as a consequence of optimal adaptation to a changing body. Nature Neuroscience 10:779-86. [aAC]
    • Körding, K. P. & Wolpert, D. M. (2004) Bayesian integration in sensorimotor learning. Nature 427(6971):244-47. doi:10.1038/nature02169. [GB]
    • Kosslyn, S. M., Thompson, W. L., Kim, I. J. & Alpert, N. M. (1995) Topographical representations of mental images in primary visual cortex. Nature 378:496- 98. [aAC]
    • Kraljic, T., Samuel, A. G. & Brennan, S. E. (2008) First impressions and last resorts: How listeners adjust to speaker variability. Psychological Science 19:332-38. [TAF]
    • Kriegstein, K. & Giraud, A. (2006) Implicit multisensory associations influence voice recognition. PLoS (Public Library of Science) Biology 4(10):e326. [aAC]
    • Kukona, A., Fang, S., Aicher, K. A., Chen, H. & Magnuson, J. S. (2011) The time course of anticipatory constraint integration. Cognition 119:23-42. [TAF]
    • Kurumada, C., Brown, M. & Tanenhaus, M. K. (2012) Pragmatic interpretation of contrastive prosody: It looks like adaptation. In: Proceedings of the 34th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, ed. N. Miyake, D. Peebles, & R. P. Cooper, pp. 647-52. Cognitive Science Society. [TAF]
    • Kveraga, K., Ghuman, A. & Bar, M. (2007) Top-down predictions in the cognitive brain. Brain and Cognition 65:145-68. [aAC]
    • Ladinig, O., Honing, H., Haden, G. & Winkler, I. (2009) Probing attentive and preattentive emergent meter in adult listeners without extensive music training. Music Perception 26:377-86. [LH]
    • Landauer, T. K. & Dumais, S. T. (1997) A solution to Plato's problem: The Latent Semantic Analysis theory of the acquisition, induction, and representation of knowledge. Psychological Review 104:211-40. [aAC]
    • Landauer, T. K., Foltz, P. W. & Laham, D. (1998) Introduction to Latent Semantic Analysis. Discourse Processes 25: 259-84. [aAC]
    • Lange, C.G. (1885/1912). The mechanisms of the emotions. In: The Classical Psychologists, ed. B. Rand. pp. 672-684. Houghton Mifflin. [AKS]
    • Langner, R., Kellermann, T., Boers, F., Sturm, W., Willmes, K. & Eickhoff, S. B. (2011) Modality-specific perceptual expectations selectively modulate baseline activity in auditory, somatosensory, and visual cortices. Cerebral Cortex 21 (12):2850-62. [aAC]
    • Large, E. W., Fink, P. & Kelso, J. A. S. (2002) Tracking simple and complex sequences. Psychological Research 66:3-17. [RSS]
    • Lee, D. & Wang, X.-J. (2009) Mechanisms for stochastic decision making in the primate frontal cortex: Single-neuron recording and circuit modeling. In: Neuroeconomics: Decision making and the brain, ed. P. Glimcher, C. Camerer, E. Fehr & R. Poldrack, pp. 481-501. Elsevier. [rAC, DRo]
    • Lee, H., Ekanadham, C. & Ng, A. (2008) Sparse deep belief net model for visual area V2. In: Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems 20 (NIPS'07), ed. J. Platt, D. Koller, Y. Singer, & S. Roweis, pp. 873-80. MIT Press. [TT]
    • Lee, M. (2010) Emergent and structured cognition in Bayesian models: Comment on Griffiths et al. and McClelland et al. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 14 (8):345-46. [aAC]
    • Lee, S. H., Blake, R. & Heeger, D. J. (2005) Traveling waves of activity in primary visual cortex during binocular rivalry. Nature Neuroscience 8 (1):22-23. [aAC]
    • Lee, T. S. & Mumford, D. (2003) Hierarchical Bayesian inference in the visual cortex. Journal of Optical Society of America, A 20(7):1434-48. [aAC]
    • Lehnert, W. (2007) Cognition, computers, and car bombs: How Yale prepared me for the 90's. In: Beliefs, reasoning, and decision making: Psycho-logic in honor of Bob Abelson, ed. R. Schank & E. Langer, pp. 143-73. Erlbaum. [aAC]
    • Lenggenhager, B., Tadi, T., Metzinger, T. & Blanke, O. (2007) Video ergo sum: Manipulating bodily self-consciousness. Science 317(5841):1096. [BP]
    • Leopold, D. & Logothetis, N. (1999) Multistable phenomena: Changing views in perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 3:254-64. [aAC]
    • Levinson, S. C. (2006) On the human “Interaction Engine.” In: Roots of human sociality, ed. N. J. Enfield & S. C. Levinson, pp. 39-69. Berg. [AR]
    • Lewis, P. A. & Miall, R. C. (2003) Distinct systems for automatic and cognitively controlled time measurement: Evidence from neuroimaging. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 13:250-55. [LH]
    • Lewis, P. A. & Miall, R. C. (2006) A right hemispheric prefrontal system for cognitive time measurement. Behavioural Processes 71:226-234. [LH]
    • Ling, S. & Carrasco, M. (2006) When sustained attention impairs perception. Nature Neuroscience 9(10):1243-45. [NB]
    • Linsker, R. (1989) An application of the principle of maximum information preservation to linear systems. In: Advances in neural information processing systems, vol. 1, ed. D. S. Touretzky, pp. 86-194. Springer. [aAC]
    • Little, D. Y. & Sommer, F. T. (2011) Learning in embodied action-perception loops through exploration. Online Publication arXive:1112.1125. [DYL]
    • Lochmann, T. & Deneve S. (2011) Neural processing as causal inference. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 21(5):774-78. [MWS]
    • Lochmann, T., Ernst, U. A. & Denève, S. (2012) Perceptual inference predicts contextual modulations of sensory responses. The Journal of Neuroscience 32 (12):4179-95. [NB]
    • Loui, P., Wessel, D. & Hudson Kam, C. L. (2010) Humans rapidly learn grammatical structure in a new musical scale. Music Perception 27:377-88. [RSS]
    • Luck, S. J. (2006) The operation of attention - millisecond by millisecond - over the first half second. In: The first half second: The microgenesis and temporal dynamics of unconscious and conscious visual processing, ed. H. Ö. B. G. Breitmeyer, pp. 187-206. MIT Press. [HB]
    • MacKay, D. J. C. (1995) Free-energy minimization algorithm for decoding and cryptoanalysis. Electron Letters 31:445-47. [aAC]
    • MacKay, D. M. (1956) The epistemological problem for automata. In: Automata studies, ed. C. E. Shannon & J. McCarthy, pp. 235-51. Princeton University Press. [aAC]
    • Madison, G. (2001) Variability in isochronous tapping: Higher-order dependencies as a function of inter tap interval. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 27:411-22. [LH]
    • Madison, G., Forsman, L., Blom, Ö., Karabanov, A. & Ullén, F. (2009) Correlations between general intelligence and components of serial timing variability. Intelligence 37:68-75. [LH]
    • Maher, B. (1988) Anomalous experience and delusional thinking: The logic of explanations. In: Delusional beliefs, ed. T. F. Oltmanns & B. A. Maher, pp. 15- 33. Wiley. [aAC]
    • Maloney, L. T. & Mamassian, P. (2009) Bayesian decision theory as a model of visual perception: Testing Bayesian transfer. Visual Neuroscience 26:147-55. [aAC]
    • Maloney, L. T. & Zhang, H. (2010) Decision-theoretic models of visual perception and action. Vision Research 50:2362-74. [aAC]
    • Mamassian, P., Landy, M. & Maloney, L. (2002) Bayesian modeling of visual perception. In: Probabilistic models of the brain, ed. R. Rao, B. Olshausen & M. Lewicki, pp. 13-36. MIT Press. [aAC]
    • Mandler, J. M. (1984) Stories, scripts, and scenes: Aspects of schema theory. Erlbaum. [JBH]
    • Mar, R. A. & Oatley, K. (2008) The function of fiction is the abstraction and simulation of social experience. Perspectives on Psychological Science 3(3):173-92. [JBH]
    • Marcus, G. (2008) Kluge: The haphazard construction of the human mind. Houghton-Mifflin. [aAC]
    • Mareschal, D., Johnson, M. H., Siros, S., Spratling, M. W., Thomas, M. S. C. & Westermann, G. (2007) Neuroconstructivism - I.: How the brain constructs cognition. Oxford University Press. [aAC, MWS]
    • Marr, D. (1982). Vision: A computational approach. Freeman. [aAC]
    • Matell, M. S. & Meck, W. H. (2004) Cortico-striatal circuits and interval timing: Coincidence detection of oscillatory processes. Cognitive Brain Research 21:139-70. [LH]
    • Mattusek, P. (1987) Studies in delusional perception (translated and condensed). In: Clinical roots of the schizophrenia concept. Translations of seminal European contributions on Schizophrenia, ed. J Cutting & M. Sheppard, pp. 87-103. Cambridge University Press. (Originally published in 1952.) [SMS]
    • McAdams, D. P. (1997) The stories we live by: Personal myths and the making of the self. The Guilford Press. [JBH]
    • McAdams, D. P. (2006) The problem of narrative coherence. Journal of Constructivist Psychology 19(2):109-25. [JBH]
    • McCarthy, J. (2008) The well-designed child. Artificial Intelligence 172(18):2003- 14. [AS]
    • McClelland, J., Botvinick, M., Noelle, D., Plaut, D., Rogers, T., Seidenberg, M. & Smith, L. (2010) Letting structure emerge: Connectionist and dynamical systems approaches to cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 14(8):348-56. [aAC]
    • McClelland, J. & Rumelhart, D. (1981) An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: Part 1. An account of basic findings. Psychological Review 88:375-407. [aAC]
    • McClelland, J., Rumelhart, D. & the PDP Research Group (1986) Parallel distributed processing, vol. 2. MIT Press. [aAC]
    • McMurray, B., Tanenhaus, M. K. & Aslin, R. N. (2009) Within-category VOT affects recovery from “lexical” garden paths: Evidence against phoneme-level inhibition. Journal of Memory and Language 60:65-91. [TAF]
    • Melloni, L., Schwiedrzik, C. M., Muller, N., Rodriguez, E. & Singer, W. (2011) Expectations change the signatures and timing of electrophysiological correlates of perceptual awareness. Journal of Neuroscience 31(4):1386-96. [aAC]
    • Menary, R. (2007) Cognitive integration: Attacking the bounds of cognition. Palgrave Macmillan. [aAC]
    • Meng, M. & Tong, F. (2004) Can attention selectively bias bistable perception? differences between binocular rivalry and ambiguous figures. Journal of Vision 4:539-51. [aAC]
    • Merker, B. (2004) Cortex, countercurrent context, and dimensional integration of lifetime memory. Cortex 40:559-76. [aAC]
    • Merker, B. H., Madison G. S. & Eckerdal, P. (2009) On the role and origin of isochrony in human rhythmic entrainment. Cortex 45:4-17. [LH]
    • Meyer, T. & Olson, C. R. (2011) Statistical learning of visual transitions in monkey inferotemporal cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 108:19401-406. [rAC, TE]
    • Meyer. L. B. (1956) Emotion and meaning in music. University of Chicago Press. [RSS]
    • Miall, R. C. (1989) The storage of time intervals using oscillating neurons. Neural Computation 1:359-71. [LH]
    • Milner, D. & Goodale, M. (2006) The visual brain in action, 2nd edition. Oxford University Press. [aAC]
    • Molnar-Szakacs, I. & Overy, K. (2006) Music and mirror neurons: From motion to 'e' motion. Social Cognition and Affective Neuroscience 1:235-41. [RSS]
    • Morrone, C. M., Ross, J. & Burr, D. (2005) Saccadic eye movements cause compression of time as well as space. Nature Neuroscience 8:950-54. [LH]
    • Muckli, L. (2010) What are we missing here? Brain imaging evidence for higher cognitive functions in primary visual cortex V1. International Journal of Imaging Systems Technology (IJIST) 20:131-39. [aAC]
    • Muckli, L., Kohler, A., Kriegeskorte, N. & Singer, W. (2005) Primary visual cortex activity along the apparent-motion trace reflects illusory perception. PLoS (Public Library of Science) Biology l3:e265. [aAC, LM]
    • Muckli, L. & Petro, L.S. (2013) Network interactions: Non-geniculate input to V1. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 23(2):195-201. [LM]
    • Mumford, D. (1992) On the computational architecture of the neocortex. II. The role of cortico-cortical loops. Biological Cybernetics 66(3):241-51. [arAC, TE, KF]
    • Mumford, D. (1994) Neuronal architectures for pattern-theoretic problems. In: Large-scale theories of the cortex, ed. C. Koch & J. Davis, pp. 125-52. MIT Press. [aAC]
    • Murray, D. J., Ellis, R. R., Bandomir, C. A. & Ross, H. E. (1999) Charpentier (1891) on the size-weight illusion. Perception and Psychophysics 61(8):1681-85. [GB]
    • Murray, S. O., Boyaci, H. & Kersten, D. (2006) The representation of perceived angular size in human primary visual cortex. Nature Reviews: Neuroscience 9:429-34. [aAC]
    • Murray, S. O., Kersten, D., Olshausen, B. A., Schrater, P. & Woods, D. L. (2002) Shape perception reduces activity in human primary visual cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 99(23):15164-69. [arAC, TE]
    • Murray, S. O., Schrater, P. & Kersten, D. (2004) Perceptual grouping and the interactions between visual cortical areas. Neural Networks 17(5-6):695-705. [aAC]
    • Musmann, H. (1979) Predictive image coding. In: Image transmission techniques, ed. W. K, Pratt, Advances in Electronics and Electron Physics, Supplement 12:73-112, Academic Press, Orlando, FL. [aAC]
    • Nagarajan, S. S., Blake, D. T., Wright, B. A., Byl, N. & Merzenich, M. M. (1998) Practice-related improvements are temporally specific but generalize across skin location, hemisphere and modality. Journal of Neuroscience 18:1559-70. [LH]
    • Nagel, S. K., Carl, C., Kringe, T., Märtin, R. & König, P. (2005) Beyond sensory substitution - learning the sixth sense. Journal of Neural Engineering 2(4):R13- R26. doi:10.1088/1741-2560/2/4/R02. [PK]
    • Narmour, E. (1990) The analysis and cognition of basic melodic structures: The implication-realization model. University of Chicago Press. [RSS]
    • Neal, R. M. & Hinton, G. (1998) A view of the EM algorithm that justifies incremental, sparse, and other variants. In: Learning in graphical models, ed. M. I. Jordan, pp. 355-68. Kluwer. [aAC]
    • Neisser, U. (1967) Cognitive psychology. Appleton-Century-Crofts. [aAC]
    • Nelson, K. (2003) Self and social functions: Individual autobiographical memory and collective narrative. Memory 11(2):125-36. [JBH]
    • Nelson, K. & Fivush, R. (2004) The emergence of autobiographical memory: A social cultural developmental theory. Psychological Review 111(2):486-511. [JBH]
    • Nelson, P. (2012) Towards a social theory of rhythm. In: Topics in musical universals/ Actualités des Universaux Musicaux, ed. J.-L. Leroy. Editions des Archives Contemporaines. [RSS]
    • Noë, A. (2004) Action in perception. MIT Press. [aAC, TF]
    • Noë, A. (2009) Out of our heads: Why you are not your brain, and other lessons from the biology of consciousness. Farrar, Straus and Giroux/Hill and Wang. [aAC, TF]
    • North, A. C. & Hargreaves, D. J. (1995) Subjective complexity, familiarity, and liking for popular music. Psychomusicology 14:77-93. [RSS]
    • Oatley, K. (1992) Best laid schemes: The psychology of emotions. Cambridge University Press. [JBH]
    • Oatley, K. (1999) Why fiction may be twice as true as fact: Fiction as cognitive and emotional simulation. Review of General Psychology 3(2):101-17. [JBH]
    • Olshausen, B. A. & Field, D. J. (1996) Emergence of simple-cell receptive field properties by learning a sparse code for natural images. Nature 381(6583):607- 609. [arAC, PK, TT]
    • Olshausen, B. A. & Field, D. J. (2005) How close are we to understanding V1? Neural Computation 17:1665-99. [MWS]
    • Overy, K. & Molnar-Szakacs, I. (2009) Being together in time: Musical experience and the mirror neuron system. Music Perception 26(5):489-504. [aAC, RSS]
    • Owen, A. M., McMillan, K. M., Laird, A. R. & Bullmore, E. (2005) N-back working memory paradigm: A meta-analysis of normative functional neuroimaging studies. Human Brain Mapping 25:46-59. [LH]
    • Oyama, S. (1999) Evolution's eye: Biology, culture and developmental systems. Duke University Press. [aAC]
    • Pack, C. C. & Born, R. T. (2001) Temporal dynamics of a neural solution to the aperture problem in visual area MT of macaque brain. Nature 409:1040-42. [aAC]
    • Palaniyappan, L. & Liddle, P. F. (2012) Does the salience network play a cardinal role in psychosis? An emerging hypothesis of insular dysfunction. Journal of Psychiatry and Neuroscience 37(1):17-27. [AKS]
    • Pascual-Leone, A. & Hamilton, R. (2001) The metamodal organization of the brain. Progress in Brain Research 134:427-45. [aAC]
    • Paulus, M. P. & Stein, M. B. (2006) An insular view of anxiety. [Review]. Biological Psychiatry 60(4):383-87. [AKS]
    • Pearce, J. M. & Hall, G. (1980) A model for Pavlovian learning: Variations in the effectiveness of conditioned but not of unconditioned stimuli. Psychological Review 87:532-52. [TE]
    • Pecenka, N. & Keller, P. E. (2011) The role of temporal prediction abilities in interpersonal sensorimotor synchronization. Experimental Brain Research 211: 505-15. [RSS]
    • Pennebaker, J. W. & Seagal, J. D. (1999) Forming a story: The health benefits of narrative. Journal of Clinical Psychology 55(10):1243-54. [JBH]
    • Peterson, J. B. (1999) Maps of meaning: The architecture of belief. Routledge. [JBH]
    • Petkova, V. I. & Ehrsson, H. H. (2008) If I were you: Perceptual illusion of body swapping. PLoS ONE 3(12):e3832. [BP]
    • Pfeifer, R., Lungarella, M., Sporns, O. & Kuniyoshi, Y. (2007) On the information theoretic implications of embodiment - principles and methods. Lecture Notes in Computer Science (LNCS), vol. 4850. Springer. [arAC]
    • Phillips, W. A. (2012) Self-organized complexity and coherent Infomax from the viewpoint of Jaynes's probability theory. Information 3(1):1-15. doi:10.3390/ info3010001. [WAP, SMS]
    • Phillips, W. A., Chapman, K. L. S. & Berry, P. D. (2004) Size perception is less context-sensitive in males. Perception 33:79-86. [WAP]
    • Phillips, W. A., Kay, J. & Smyth, D. (1995) The discovery of structure by multistream networks of local processors with contextual guidance. Network: Computation in Neural Systems 6:225-46. [rAC, PK, WAP]
    • Phillips, W. A. & Silverstein, S. M. (2003) Convergence of biological and psychological perspectives on cognitive coordination in schizophrenia. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 26:65-82; discussion 82-137. [WAP, SMS]
    • Phillips, W. A. & Singer, W. (1997) In search of common foundations for cortical computation. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 20:657-722. [WAP, MWS]
    • Phillips, W. A., von der Malsburg, C. & Singer, W. (2010) Dynamic coordination in brain and mind. In: Strüngmann Forum Report, vol. 5: Dynamic coordination in the brain: From neurons to mind, ed. C. von der Malsburg, W. A. Phillips & W. Singer, Chapter 1, pp. 1-24. MIT Press. [rAC, WAP]
    • Phillips-Silver J. & Trainor, L. J. (2007) Hearing what the body feels: Auditory encoding of rhythmic movement. Cognition 105:533-46. [LH]
    • Phillips-Silver, J. & Trainor, L. J. (2008) Vestibular influence on auditory metrical interpretation. Brain and Cognition 67:94-102. [RSS]
    • Piaget, J. (1952) The origins of intelligence in children. International University Press. [PK]
    • Pickering, M. J. & Garrod, S. (2007) Do people use language production to make predictions during comprehension? Trends in Cognitive Sciences (11):105- 110. [arAC]
    • Platt, M. & Glimcher, P. (1999) Neural correlates of decision variables in parietal cortex. Nature 400:233-38. [DRo]
    • Ploghaus, A., Tracey, I., Gati, J. S., Clare, S., Menon, R. S., Matthews, P. M. & Rawlins, J. N. (1999) Dissociating pain from its anticipation in the human brain. Science 284(5422):1979-81. [AKS]
    • Posner, M. (1980) Orienting of attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 32:33. [rAC]
    • Pouget, A., Dayan, P. & Zemel, R. (2003) Inference and computation with population codes. Annual Review of Neuroscience 26:381-410. [aAC]
    • Powers, W. T. (1973) Behavior, the control of perception. Aldine de Gruyter. [AS]
    • Pribram, K. H. (1971) Languages of the brain. Prentice-Hall. [BB]
    • Pribram, K. H. (1980) The orienting reaction: Key to brain representational mechanisms. In: The orienting reflex in humans, ed. H. D. Kimmel, pp. 3-20. Erlbaum. [aAC]
    • Prinz, J. J. (2005) A neurofunctional theory of consciousness. In: Cognition and the brain: Philosophy and neuroscience movement, ed. A. Brook & K. Akins, pp. 381-96. Cambridge University Press. [aAC]
    • Proulx, T., Inzlicht, M. & Harmon-Jones, E. (2012) Understanding all inconsistency compensation as a palliative response to violated expectations. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 16(5):285-91. [JBH]
    • Purves, D. & Lotto, R. B. (2003) Why we see what we do: An empirical theory of vision. Sinauer. [aAC]
    • Quine, W. V. O. (1951) Two dogmas of empiricism. The Philosophical Review 60:20- 43. [MLA]
    • Rammsayer, T. (1999) Neuropharmacological evidence for different timing mechanisms in humans. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, Section B: Comparative and Physiological Psychology 52:273-86. [LH]
    • Rao, R. P. N. & Ballard, D. H. (1999) Predictive coding in the visual cortex: A functional interpretation of some extra-classical receptive-field effects. Nature Neuroscience 2(1):79-87. [aAC, TE, KF, LM, MWS]
    • Rao, R. P. N. & Sejnowski, T. J. (2002) Predictive coding, cortical feedback, and spike-timing dependent plasticity. In: Probabilistic models of the brain: Perception and neural function, ed. R. P. N. Rao, B. A. Olshausen & M. S. Lewicki, pp. 297-315. MIT Press. [aAC]
    • Rauss, K., Schwartz, S. & Pourtois, G. (2011) Top-down effects on early visual processing in humans: A predictive coding framework. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 35(5):1237-53. [aAC]
    • Read, J., Perry, B. D., Moskowitz, A. & Connolly, J. (2001) The contribution of early traumatic events to schizophrenia in some patients: A traumagenic neurodevelopmental model. Psychiatry 64: 319-45. [SMS]
    • Read, J., van Os, J., Morrison, A. P. & Ross, C. A. (2005) Childhood trauma, psychosis and schizophrenia: A literature review with theoretical and clinical implications. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 112:330-50. [SMS]
    • Reddy, L., Tsuchiya, N. & Serre, T. (2010) Reading the mind's eye: decoding category information during mental imagery. NeuroImage 50(2):818-25. [aAC]
    • Reich, L., Szwed, M., Cohen, L. & Amedi, A. (2011) A ventral stream reading center independent of visual experience. Current Biology 21:363-68. [aAC]
    • Reichert, D., Seriès, P. & Storkey, A. (2010) Hallucinations in Charles Bonnet Syndrome induced by homeostasis: A Deep Boltzmann Machine model. Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems 23:2020-28. [rAC]
    • Repp, B. (1999) Detecting deviations from metronomic timing in music: Effects of perceptual structure on the mental timekeeper. Perception and Psychophysics 61(3):529-48. [RSS]
    • Ricoeur, P., Blamey, K. & Pellauer, D. (1990) Time and narrative, vol. 3. University of Chicago Press. [JBH]
    • Rieke, F. (1999) Spikes: Exploring the neural code, MIT Press. [aAC]
    • Riesenhuber, M. & Poggio, T (2000) Models of object recognition. Nature Neuroscience 3(Suppl.):1199-204. [TE]
    • Robbins, H. (1956) An empirical Bayes approach to statistics. In: Proceedings of the Third Berkeley Symposium on Mathematical Statistics and Probability, vol. 1: Contributions to the Theory of Statistics, pp. 157-63. University of California Press. [aAC]
    • Roepstorff, A. (2008) Things to think with: Words and objects as material symbols. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, B 363(1499):2049-54. [AR]
    • Roepstoff, A. & Frith, C. (2012) Neuroanthropology or simply anthropology? Going experimental as method, as object of study, and as research aesthetic. Anthropological Theory 12(1):101-11. [AR]
    • Roepstorff, A., Niewohner, J. & Beck, S. (2010) Enculturing brains through patterned practices. Neural Networks 23(8-9):1051-59. [arAC, AR]
    • Rorty, R. (1979) Philosophy and the mirror of nature. Princeton University Press. [MLA]
    • Ross, D., Sharp, C., Vuchinich, R. & Spurrett, D. (2008) Midbrain mutiny: The picoeconomics and neuroeconomics of disordered gambling. MIT Press. [DRo]
    • Ross, H. E. (1969) When is a weight not illusory? The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 21(4):346-55. doi:10.1080/14640746908400230. [GB]
    • Rowlands, M. (1999) The body in mind: Understanding cognitive processes. Cambridge University Press. [aAC]
    • Rowlands, M (2006) Body language: Representing in action. MIT Press. [aAC]
    • Rumelhart, D. E., McClelland, J. L. & the PDP Research Group (1986) Parallel distributed processing, vol. I: Explorations in the microstructure of cognition. Foundations. MIT Press. [aAC]
    • Rust, N. C., Schwartz, O., Movshon, J. A. & Simoncelli, E. P. (2005) Spatiotemporal elements of Macaque V1 receptive fields. Neuron 46:945-56. [TT]
    • Sachs, E. (1967) Dissociation of learning in rats and its similarities to dissociative states in man. In: Comparative psychopathology: Animal and human, ed. J. Zubin & H. Hunt, pp. 249-304. Grune and Stratton. [aAC]
    • Sadakata, M., Desain, P. & Honing, H. (2006) The Bayesian way to relate rhythm perception and production. Music Perception 23:269-88. [RSS]
    • Salakhutdinov, R. & Hinton, G. E. (2009) Deep Boltzmann machines. Proceedings of the 12th International Conference on Artificial Intelligence and Statistics (AISTATS), vol. 5, ed. D. van Dyk & M. Welling, pp. 448-55. The Journal of Machine Learning Research, published online, at http://jmlr.csail.mit.edu/ proceedings/papers/v5/ [rAC]
    • Sanders, L. L., Muckli, L., de Millas, W., Lautenschlager, M., Heinz, A., Kathmann, N. & Sterzer, P. (2012) Detection of visual events along the apparent motion trace in patients with paranoid schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research. [Epub ahead of print: April 28, 2012]. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/ j.psychres.2012.03.006. [LM]
    • Santhouse, A. M., Howard, R. J. & ffytche, D. H. (2000) Visual hallucinatory syndromes and the anatomy of the visual brain. Brain 123:2055-64. [SMS]
    • Sarbin, T. R. (1986) Narrative psychology: The storied nature of human conduct. Praeger/Greenwood. [JBH]
    • Sass, L. (1992) Madness and modernism. Insanity in the light of modern art, literature and thought. Basic Books. [SMS]
    • Saxe, A., Bhand, M., Mudur, R., Suresh, B. & Ng, A. (2011) Modeling cortical representational plasticity with unsupervised feature learning. Poster presented at COSYNE 2011, Salt Lake City, Utah, February 24-27, 2011. Available at: http://www.stanford.edu/~asaxe/papers. [TT]
    • Schachter, S. & Singer, J. E. (1962) Cognitive, social, and physiological determinants of emotional state. Psychological Review 69:379-99. [AKS]
    • Schaefer, R. S., Vlek, R. J. & Desain, P. (2011a) Decomposing rhythm processing: Electroencephalography of perceived and self-imposed rhythmic patterns. Psychological Research 75(2):95-106. [RSS]
    • Schaefer, R. S., Vlek, R. J. & Desain, P. (2011b) Music perception and imagery in EEG: Alpha band effects of task and stimulus. International Journal for Psychophysiology 82(3):254-59. [RSS]
    • Schank, R. & Abelson, R. (1977) Scripts, plans, goals and understanding: An inquiry into human knowledge structures. Erlbaum. [JBH]
    • Schenk, T. & McIntosh, R. (2010) Do we have independent visual streams for perception and action? Cognitive Neuroscience 1:52-78. [rAC]
    • Schultz, W., Dayan, P. & Montague, P. R. (1997) A neural substrate of prediction and reward. Science 275:1593-99. [DRo]
    • Schwartz, O., Hsu, A. & Dayan, P. (2007) Space and time in visual context Nature Reviews Neuroscience 8:522-35. [aAC]
    • Segall, M. H., Campbell, D. T. & Herskovits, M. J. (1963) Cultural differences in the perception of geometric illusions. Science 139(3556):769-71. [PK]
    • Sellars, W. (1962) Philosophy and the scientific image of man. In: Frontiers of Science and Philosophy, ed. R. G. Colodny, pp. 35-78. University of Pittsburgh Press. [Reprinted in: Science, Perception and Reality by W. Sellars (1963, Routledge & Kegan Paul)]. [aAC, DCD]
    • Sellars, W. (1963) Science, perception, and reality. Routledge & Kegan Paul. [JBH]
    • Seth, A. K., Suzuki, K. & Critchley, H. D. (2011) An interoceptive predictive coding model of conscious presence. Frontiers in Psychology 2:395. [rAC, AKS]
    • Shams, L., Ma, W. J. & Beierholm, U. (2005) Sound-induced flash illusion as an optimal percept. NeuroReport 16(10):1107-10. [aAC]
    • Shi, Yun Q. & Sun, H. (1999) Image and video compression for multimedia engineering: Fundamentals, algorithms, and standards. CRC Press. [aAC]
    • Silverstein, S. M., Berten, S., Essex, B., Kovács, I., Susmaras, T. & Little, D. M. (2009) An fMRI examination of visual integration in schizophrenia. Journal of Integrative Neuroscience 8:175-202. [SMS]
    • Silverstein, S. M. & Keane, B. P. (2011) Perceptual organization impairment in schizophrenia and associated brain mechanisms: Review of research from 2005 to 2010. Schizophrenia Bulletin 37:690-99. [SMS]
    • Simoncelli, E. P. & Olshausen, B. A. (2001) Natural image statistics and neural representation. Annual Review of Neuroscience 24:1193-216. [PK]
    • Simons, J. S., Schölvinck, M. L., Gilbert, S. J., Frith, C. D., & Burgess, P. W. (2006) Differential components of prospective memory? Evidence from fMRI. Neuropsychologia 4:1388-97. [LH]
    • Singer, T., Critchley, H. D. & Preuschoff, K. (2009) A common role of insula in feelings, empathy and uncertainty. Trends in Cognition Science 13(8):334-40. [AKS]
    • Singer, W. (1995) Development and plasticity of cortical processing architectures. Science 270:758-64. [SMS]
    • Sloman, A. (1971) Interactions between philosophy and AI: The role of intuition and non-logical reasoning in intelligence. In: Proceedings of the 2nd IJCAI [International Joint Conference on Artificial Intelligence], ed. D. C. Cooper, pp. 209- 26. William Kaufmann. Available at: http://www.cs.bham.ac.uk/research/cogaff/ 04.html#200407. [AS]
    • Sloman, A. (1978) The computer revolution in philosophy. Harvester Press/Humanities Press. [AS]
    • Sloman, A. (1979) The primacy of non-communicative language. In: The analysis of meaning: Informatics 5, Proceedings ASLIB/BCS Conference, Oxford, March 1979, ed. M. MacCafferty & K. Gray, pp. 1-15. Aslib. Available at: http://www. cs.bham.ac.uk/research/projects/cogaff/81-95.html#43. [AS]
    • Sloman, A. (1982) Image interpretation: The way ahead? In: Physical and biological processing of images (Proceedings of an International Symposium organised by The Rank Prize Funds, London, 1982), ed. O. Braddick & A. Sleigh, pp. 380- 401. Springer-Verlag. Available at: http://www.cs.bham.ac.uk/research/projects/ cogaff/06.html#0604. [AS]
    • Sloman, A. (1987) Reference without causal links. In: Advances in artificial intelligence, vol. 2, ed. J. B. H. du Boulay, D. Hogg, & L. Steels, pp. 369-81. NorthHolland. Available at: http://www.cs.bham.ac.uk/research/projects/cogaff/81-95. html#5. [AS]
    • Sloman, A. (1989) On designing a visual system (towards a Gibsonian computational model of vision). Journal of Experimental and Theoretical AI 1(4):289-37. Available at: http://www.cs.bham.ac.uk/research/projects/cogaff/81-95. html#7. [AS]
    • Sloman, A. (1990) Must intelligent systems be scruffy? In: Evolving knowledge in natural science and artificial intelligence, ed. J. E. Tiles, G. T. McKee & G. C. Dean. Pitman. [aAC]
    • Sloman, A. (1993) The mind as a control system. In: Philosophy and the cognitive sciences, ed. C. Hookway & D. Peterson, pp. 69-110. Cambridge University Press. Available at: http://www.cs.bham.ac.uk/research/projects/cogaff/81-95. html#18. [AS]
    • Sloman, A. (1996) Actual possibilities. In: Principles of knowledge representation and reasoning: Proceedings of the 5th International Conference (KR '96), ed. L. Aiello & S. Shapiro, pp. 627-38. Morgan Kaufmann. [AS]
    • Sloman, A. (2002) Diagrams in the mind. In: Diagrammatic representation and reasoning, ed. M. Anderson, B. Meyer & P. Olivier, pp. 7-28. SpringerVerlag. [AS]
    • Sloman, A. (2006) Requirements for a fully deliberative architecture (or component of an architecture). Research Note No. COSY-DP-0604, May 2006. School of Computer Science, University of Birmingham, UK. [AS]
    • Sloman, A. (2008) A multi-picture challenge for theories of vision. Research Note No. COSY-PR-0801. School of Computer Science, University of Birmingham, UK. [AS]
    • Sloman, A. (2009) Some requirements for human-like robots: Why the recent overemphasis on embodiment has held up progress. In: Creating brain-like intelligence, ed. B. Sendhoff, E. Koerner, O. Sporns, H. Ritter & K. Doya, pp. 248-77. Springer-Verlag. [AS]
    • Sloman, A. (2010) If learning maths requires a teacher, where did the first teachers come from? In: Proceedings of the International Symposium on Mathematical Practice and Cognition, AISB 2010 Convention, De Montfort University, Leicester, ed. A. Pease, M. Guhe & A. Smaill, pp. 30-39. AISB (Society for the Study of Artificial Intelliegence and Simulation of Behaviour). [AS]
    • Sloman, A. (2011a) Varieties of meta-cognition in natural and artificial systems. In: Metareasoning: Thinking about thinking, ed. M. T. Cox & A. Raja, pp. 307-23. MIT Press. [AS]
    • Sloman, A. (2011b) What's vision for, and how does it work? From Marr (and earlier) to Gibson and beyond. (Online tutorial presentation, September 2011. Available at: http://www.slideshare.net/asloman/). [AS]
    • Sloman, A. (2012 ) Paper 4: Virtual machinery and evolution of mind (Part 3). Metamorphogenesis: Evolution of information-processing machinery. In: Alan Turing - his work and impact, ed. S. B. Cooper & J. van Leeuwen. Elsevier. [AS]
    • Smith, F. W. & Muckli, L. (2010) Nonstimulated early visual areas carry information about surrounding context. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 16:20099-103. [aAC, LM]
    • Smith, L. & Gasser, M. (2005) The development of embodied cognition: Six lessons from babies. Artificial Life 11(1):13-30. [aAC]
    • Smith, P. L. & Ratcliff, R. (2004) Psychology and neurobiology of simple decisions. Trends in Neuroscience 27:161-68. [aAC]
    • Sokolov, E. N. (1960) Neuronal models and the orienting reflex. In: The central nervous system and behavior, ed. M. Brazier, pp. 187-276. Josiah Macy Jr. Foundation. [BB, aAC]
    • Sporns, O. (2007) What neuro-robotic models can teach us about neural and cognitive development. In: Neuroconstructivism: Perspectives and prospects, Vol. 2, ed. D. Mareschal, S. Sirois, G. Westermann & M. H. Johnson, pp. 179-204. Oxford University Press. [aAC]
    • Spratling, M. W. (2008a) Predictive coding as a model of biased competition in visual attention. Vision Research 48(12):1391-408. [aAC, WAP, MWS]
    • Spratling, M. W. (2008b) Reconciling predictive coding and biased competition models of cortical function. Frontiers in Computational Neuroscience 2(4):1-8. [LM, MWS]
    • Spratling, M. W. (2010) Predictive coding as a model of response properties in cortical area V1. Journal of Neuroscience 30(9):3531-543. [MWS]
    • Spratling, M. W. (2011) A single functional model accounts for the distinct properties of suppression in cortical area V1. Vision Research 51(6):563-76. [MWS]
    • Spratling, M. W. (2012a) Predictive coding accounts for V1 response properties recorded using reverse correlation. Biological Cybernetics 106(1):37-49. [MWS]
    • Spratling, M. W. (2012b) Unsupervised learning of generative and discriminative weights encoding elementary image components in a predictive coding model of cortical function. Neural Computation 24(1):60-103. [MWS]
    • Srinivasan, M. V., Laughlin, S. B. & Dubs A. (1982) Predictive coding: A fresh view of inhibition in the retina. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, B 216:427-59. [aAC]
    • Staum Casasanto, L. (2008) Does social information influence sentence processing? In: Proceedings of the 30th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, ed. B. C. Love, K. McRae, & V. M. Sloutsky, pp. 799-804. Cognitive Science Society. [TAF]
    • Stephan, K., Friston, K. & Frith, C. (2009) Dysconnection in schizophrenia: From abnormal synaptic plasticity to failures of self-monitoring. Schizophrenia Bulletin 35(3):509-27. [rAC]
    • Sterelny, K. (2003) Thought in a hostile world: The evolution of human cognition, Blackwell. [aAC]
    • Sterelny, K. (2007) Social intelligence, human intelligence and niche construction. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Series B: Biological Sciences 362(1480):719-30. [aAC]
    • Still, S. (2009) Information-theoretic approach to interactive learning. Europhysics Letters 85:28005. [DYL]
    • Störmer, V., McDonald, J. & Hillyard, S. A. (2009) Cross-modal cueing of attention alters appearance and early cortical processing of visual stimuli. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 106(52):22456-61. [NB]
    • Stotz, K. (2010) Human nature and cognitive-developmental niche construction. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences 9(4):483-501. [aAC]
    • Summerfield, C. & Egner, T (2009) Expectation (and attention) in visual cognition. Trends in Cognition Science 13:403-409. [aAC, TE]
    • Summerfield, C., Egner, T., Greene, M., Koechlin, E., Mangels, J. & Hirsch, J (2006) Predictive codes for forthcoming perception in the frontal cortex. Science 314:1311-14. [TE]
    • Summerfield, C. & Koechlin, E. (2008) A neural representation of prior information during perceptual inference. Neuron 59:336-47. [TE]
    • Summerfield, C., Trittschuh, E. H., Monti, J. M., Mesulam, M. M. & Egner, T. (2008) Neural repetition suppression reflects fulfilled perceptual expectations. Nature Neuroscience 11(9):1004-1006. [aAC, TE]
    • Summerfield, C., Wyart, V., Johnen, V. M. & De Gardelle, V (2011) Human scalp electroencephalography reveals that repetition suppression varied with expectation. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 5:67. (Online publication). doi:10.3389/fnhum.2011.00067. [TE]
    • Switkes, E., Mayer, M. J. & Sloan, J. A. (1978) Spatial frequency analysis of the visual environment: Anisotropy and the carpentered environment hypothesis. Vision Research 18:1393-99. [BB]
    • Synofzik, M., Thier, P., Leube, D. T., Schlotterbeck, P. & Lindner, A. (2010) Misattributions of agency in schizophrenia are based on imprecise predictions about the sensory consequences of one's actions. Brain 133(Pt. 1):262-71. [AKS]
    • Tanaka, K. (1996) Inferotemporal cortex and object vision. Annual Review of Neuroscience 19:109-39. [PK]
    • Tanenhaus, M. K. & Hare, M. (2007) Phonological typicality and sentence processing. Trends in Cognitive Science 11:93-95. [TAF]
    • Temperley, D. (2004) Communicative pressure and the evolution of musical styles. Music Perception 21:313-37. [RSS]
    • Temperley, D. (2007) Music and probability. The MIT Press. [RSS]
    • Thelen, E. & Smith, L. (1994) A dynamic systems approach to the development of cognition and action. MIT Press. [aAC]
    • Thompson, E. (2007) Mind in life: Biology, phenomenology, and the sciences of mind. Harvard University Press. [aAC]
    • Tishby, N., Pereira, F. C. & Bialek, W. (1999) The information bottleneck method. In: Proceedings of the 37th Allerton Conference on Communication, Control, and Computing, ed. B. Hajek & R. S. Sreenivas, pp. 368-77. University of Illinois Press. [DYL]
    • Todorov, E. (2004) Optimality principles in sensorimotor control. Nature Neuroscience 7(9):907-15. [NS]
    • Todorov, E. (2006) Optimal control theory. In: Bayesian brain, ed. K. Doya, pp. 269-98. MIT Press. [NS]
    • Todorov, E. (2009) Parallels between sensory and motor information processing. In: The cognitive neurosciences, 4th edition, ed. M. Gazzaniga, pp. 613-24. MIT Press. [aAC]
    • Todorov, E. & Jordan, M. I. (2002) Optimal feedback control as a theory of motor coordination. Nature Neuroscience 5(11):1226-35. [aAC, NS]
    • Todorovic, A., van Ede, F., Maris, E. & de Lange, F. P. (2011) Prior expectation mediates neural adaptation to repeated sounds in the auditory cortex: An MEG study. Journal of Neuroscience 31:9118-23. [TE]
    • Tong, F., Meng, M. & Blake, R. (2006) Neural bases of binocular rivalry. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 10:502-11. [MLA]
    • Toussaint, M. (2009) Probabilistic inference as a model of planned behavior. Künstliche Intelligenz 3:23-29. [aAC]
    • Townsend, B. R., Paninski, L. & Lemon, R. N. (2006) Linear encoding of muscle activity in primary motor cortex and cerebellum. Journal of Neurophysiology 96 (5): 2578-92. [DRa]
    • Trehub, A. (1991) The cognitive brain. MIT Press. Available at: http://www.people. umass.edu/trehub/. [AS]
    • Tribus, M. (1961) Thermodynamics and thermostatics: An introduction to energy, information and states of matter, with engineering applications. D. Van Nostrand. [aAC, KF]
    • Tudusciuc, O. & Nieder, A. (2009) Contributions of primate prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices to length and numerosity representation. Journal of Neurophysiology 101(6):2984-94. [DRa]
    • Turing, A. M. (1952) The chemical basis of morphogenesis. Philosophical Transactions of Royal Society of London B 237:37-72. [AS]
    • Uhlhaas, P. J. & Mishara, A. L. (2007) Perceptual anomalies in schizophrenia: Integrating phenomenology and cognitive neuroscience. Schizophrenia Bulletin 33:142-56. [SMS]
    • Ungerleider, L. G. & Mishkin, M. (1982) Two cortical visual systems. In: Analysis of visual behavior, ed. D. Ingle, M. A. Goodale & R. J. Mansfield, pp. 549-86. MIT Press. [KF]
    • Van Essen, D. C. (2005) Corticocortical and thalamocortical information flow in the primate visual system. Progress in Brain Research 149:173-85. [LM]
    • Van Voorhis, S. & Hillyard, S. A. (1977) Visual evoked potentials and selective attention to points in space. Perception and Psychophysics 22(1):54-62. [HB]
    • Varela, F. J. (1999) The specious present: A neurophenomenology of time consciousness. In: Naturalizing phenomenology: Issues in contemporary phenomenology and cognitive science, ed. J. Petitot, F. J. Varela, B. Pachoud & J.-M. Roy, pp. 266-317. Stanford University Press. [TF]
    • Varela, F. J., Lachaux, J.-P., Rodriguez, E. & Martinerie, J. (2001) The brainweb: Phase synchronization and large-scale integration. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 2:229-39. [TF]
    • Varela, F. J., Thompson, E. & Rosch, E. (1991) The embodied mind. MIT Press. [aAC]
    • Velleman, J. D. (1989) Practical reflection. Princeton University Press. [aAC]
    • Verschure, P., Voegtlin, T. & Douglas, R. (2003) Environmentally mediated synergy between perception and behaviour in mobile robots. Nature 425:620-24. [aAC]
    • Vetter, P., Edwards, G. & Muckli, L. (2012) Transfer of predictive signals across saccades. Frontiers in Psychology 3 (176):1-10. [LM]
    • Vetter, P., Grosbras, M. H. & Muckli, L. (under revision) TMS over V5 disrupts motion predictability. [LM]
    • Vilares, I. & Körding, K. (2011) Bayesian models: The structure of the world, uncertainty, behavior, and the brain. Annals of the New York Academy of Science 1224:22-39. [aAC]
    • Villalon-Turrubiates, I. E., Andrade-Lucio, J. A. & Ibarra-Manzano, O. G. (2004) Multidimensional digital signal estimation using Kalman's theory for computeraided applications. In: Proceedings of the International Conference on Computing, Communications, and Control Technologies, Austin, Texas, August 14-17, 2004 (CCCT Proceedings, Vol. 7), ed. H.-W. Chu, pp. 48-53. University of Texas Press. [DRa]
    • Vinje, W. E. & Gallant J. L. (2000) Sparse coding and decorrelation in primary visual cortex during natural vision. Science 287:1273-76. [TT]
    • von der Malsburg, C., Phillips, W. A. & Singer, W., eds. (2010) Strungmann Forum Report, Vol. 5. Dynamic coordination in the brain: From neurons to mind. MIT Press. [rAC, WAP]
    • von Uexküll, J. (1934/1957) A stroll through the worlds of animals and men: A picture book of invisible worlds. In: Instinctive behavior: The development of a modern concept, ed. & trans. C. H. Schiller. International Universities Press (1957). [DCD]
    • Vuust P. & Frith, C. D. (2008) Anticipation is the key to understanding music and the effects of music on emotion. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 31:599-600. [RSS]
    • Wacongne, C., Changeux, J. P. & Dehaene, S. (2012) A neuronal model of predictive coding accounting for the mismatch negativity. Journal of Neuroscience 32:3665-78. [TE]
    • Waelti, P., Dickinson, A. & Schultz, W. (2001) Dopamine responses comply with basic assumptions of formal learning theory. Nature 412:43-48. [aAC]
    • Waydo, S., Kraskov, A., Quiroga, R. Q., Fried, I. & Koch, C. (2006) Sparse representation in the human medial temporal lobe. Journal of Neuroscience 26:10232-34. [TT]
    • Weber J. (2002) The judgement of the eye. The metamorphoses of geometry - one of the sources of visual perception and consciousness (a further development of Gestalt psychology). Springer. [SMS]
    • Weiss, Y., Simoncelli, E. P. & Adelson, E. H. (2002) Motion illusions as optimal percepts. Nature Neuroscience 5(6):598-604. doi:10.1038/nn858. [aAC, GB]
    • Wells, J. B., Christiansen, M. H., Race, D. S., Acheson, D. J. & MacDonald, M. C. (2009) Experience and sentence comprehension: Statistical learning and relative clause comprehension. Cognitive Psychology 58:250-71. [TAF]
    • Wheeler, M. (2005) Reconstructing the cognitive world. MIT Press. [aAC]
    • Wheeler, M. & Clark, A. (2009) Culture, embodiment and genes: Unravelling the triple helix. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, B 363 (1509):3563-75. [aAC]
    • Wilson, R. A. (1994) Wide computationalism. Mind 103:351-72. [aAC]
    • Wilson, R. A. (2004) Boundaries of the mind: The individual in the fragile sciences - cognition. Cambridge University Press. [aAC]
    • Womelsdorf, T., Anton-Erxleben, K., Pieper, F. & Treue, S. (2006) Dynamic shifts of visual receptive fields in cortical area MT by spatial attention. Nature Neuroscience 9:1156-60. [NB]
    • Wu, Z. (1985) Multidimensional state space model Kalman filtering with applications to image restoration. IEEE Transactions on Acoustics, Speech, and Signal Processing 33:1576-92. [DRa]
    • Wyart, V., Nobre. A. C. & Summerfield, C. (2012) Dissociable prior influences of signal probability and relevance on visual contrast sensitivity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 109:3593-98. [rAC, TE]
    • Wyss, R., König, P. & Verschure, P. F. M. J. (2004) Involving the motor system in decision making. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, B: Biological Sciences 271(Suppl. 3):S50-52. [PK]
    • Yeshurun, Y. & Carrasco, M. (1998) Attention improves or impairs visual performance by enhancing spatial resolution. Nature 396:72-75. [NB]
    • Yu, A. J. (2007) Adaptive behavior: Humans act as Bayesian learners. Current Biology 17:R977-80. [aAC]
    • Yuille, A. & Kersten, D. (2006) Vision as Bayesian inference: Analysis by synthesis? Trends in Cognitive Science 10(7):301-308. [aAC, KF]
    • Zahedi, K., Ay, N. & Der, R. (2010) Higher coordination with less control - a result of information maximization in the sensorimotor loop. Adaptive Behavior 18(3-4):338-55. [aAC]
    • Zhu, Q. & Bingham, G. P. (2011) Human readiness to throw: The size-weight illusion is not an illusion when picking the best objects to throw. Evolution and Human Behavior 32(4):288-93. [rAC]
  • No similar publications.

Share - Bookmark

Cite this article