LOGIN TO YOUR ACCOUNT

Username
Password
Remember Me
Or use your Academic/Social account:

CREATE AN ACCOUNT

Or use your Academic/Social account:

Congratulations!

You have just completed your registration at OpenAire.

Before you can login to the site, you will need to activate your account. An e-mail will be sent to you with the proper instructions.

Important!

Please note that this site is currently undergoing Beta testing.
Any new content you create is not guaranteed to be present to the final version of the site upon release.

Thank you for your patience,
OpenAire Dev Team.

Close This Message

CREATE AN ACCOUNT

Name:
Username:
Password:
Verify Password:
E-mail:
Verify E-mail:
*All Fields Are Required.
Please Verify You Are Human:
fbtwitterlinkedinvimeoflicker grey 14rssslideshare1
Wilcockson, Thomas; Pothos, Emmanuel; Fawcett, Angela (2016)
Types: Article
Subjects: RC
Background: A number of cognitive deficits are associated with dyslexia. However, only a limited amount of research has been performed exploring a putative link between dyslexia and substance use. As substance use is thought to involve a cognitive component, it is possible that the pattern of substance use would be different for dyslexic participants, when compared to nondyslexic controls. During the current study, a guiding hypothesis was that people with dyslexia would demonstrate less substance use than nondyslexic controls. Theories of memory activation, automaticity, and attentional bias in substance use suggest that cognitive components of substance use are important in the development and maintenance of continued substance use and it is thought that, at least some of these components, would be impaired in a dyslexic population.\ud \ud Objectives: If the cognitive deficits displayed by dyslexics somehow impair the development of cognitive components of substance use, substance use for dyslexic participants may be less pronounced. This paper therefore examines this hypothesis by comparing substance use within dyslexic and nondyslexic participants, from an undergraduate population.\ud \ud Methods: This was an exploratory questionnaire-based study. Dyslexic participants (n = 35) were compared to control participants (n = 62) on a series of questions designed to measure their substance use history.\ud \ud Results: The results provided preliminary evidence of a difference between dyslexic and nondyslexic substance use. Dyslexics reported a substance use history that was significantly lower than nondyslexic controls. \ud \ud Conclusions/Importance: These results are interpreted in terms of cognitive deficits within dyslexia and with reference to the cognitive model of substance use.
  • The results below are discovered through our pilot algorithms. Let us know how we are doing!

    • Barrett, S. P., Darredeau, C., & Pihl, R. O. (2006). Patterns of simultaneous polysubstance use in drug using university students. Human Psychopharmacology: Clinical and Experimental, 21(4), 255-263.
    • Becker, J. B., & Hu, M. (2008). Sex differences in drug abuse. Frontiers in neuroendocrinology, 29(1), 36-47.
    • Bewick, B. M., Mulhern, B., Barkham, M., Trusler, K., Hill, A. J., & Stiles, W. B. (2008). Changes in undergraduate student alcohol consumption as they progress through university. BMC Public Health, 8(1), 163.
    • Bishop, D. V. M. (2002). Cerebellar Abnormalities In Developmental Dyslexia: Cause, Correlate Or Consequence? Cortex, 38(4), 491-498.
    • Bradley, L., & Bryant, P. E. (1983). Categorizing sounds and learning to read-a causal connection. Nature, 301, 419-421.
    • Casco, C., Tressoldi, P.E. And Dellantonio, A., (1998). Visual Selective Attention And Reading Efficiency Are Related In Children. Cortex 34, 531-546.
    • Cox, W. M., Fadardi, J. S., & Pothos, E. M. (2006). The Addiction-Stroop Test: Theoretical Considerations And Procedural Recommendations. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 443- 476.
    • Cox, W. M., Pothos, E. M., Hosier, S. G. (2007) Cognitive-Motivational Predictors Of Excessive Drinkers' Success In Changing. Psychopharmacology 192, 499-510.
    • Critchley, M. And Critchley, E.A. (1978) Dyslexia Defined. Heinemann: London.
    • D├ąderman, A. M., Meurling, A. W., & Levander, S. (2012). 'Speedy Action over Goal Orientation': Cognitive Impulsivity in Male Forensic Patients with Dyslexia. Dyslexia, 18(4), 226-235.
    • Eden, G. F., & Zeffiro, T. A. (1998). Neural systems affected in developmental dyslexia revealed by functional neuroimaging. Neuron, 21(2), 279-282.
    • Ewing, J. A. (1984). Detecting Alcoholism - The Cage Questionnaire. Jama-Journal Of The American Medical Association, 252(14), 1905-1907.
    • Facoetti, A., Paganoni, P., Turatto, M., Marzola, V., & Mascetti, G. G. (2000). Visual-Spatial Attention In Developmental Dyslexia. Cortex, 36(1), 109-123.
    • Frederickson, N., & Jacobs, S. (2001). Controllability Attributions For Academic Performance And The Perceived Scholastic Competence, Global Self-Worth And Achievement Of Children With Dyslexia. School Psychology International, 22(4), 401-416.
    • Frith, U., Landerl, K., & Frith, C. (1995). Dyslexia and verbal fluency: More evidence for a phonological deficit. DYSLEXIA-CHICHESTER-, 1, 2-11.
    • Grigorenko, E. L. (2006). Learning disabilities in juvenile offenders. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 15(2), 353-371.
    • Hammersley, R., Marsland, L., & Reid, M. (2003). Substance Use By Young Offenders: The Impact Of The Normalisation Of Drug Use In The Early Years Of The 21st Century. Home Office Research Study 261. London: Home Office Research And Statistics Directorate.
    • Jensen, J., Lindgren, M., Meurling, A. W., Ingvar, D. H., & Levander, S. (1999). Dyslexia among Swedish prison inmates in relation to neuropsychology and personality. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 5(05), 452-461.
    • Kirk, J., & Reid, G. (2001). An Examination Of The Relationship Between Dyslexia And Offending In Young People And The Implications For The Training System. Dyslexia, 7(2), 77-84.
    • Kirk, J., & Reid, G. (2001). An Examination Of The Relationship Between Dyslexia And Offending In Young People And The Implications For The Training System. Dyslexia, 7(2), 77-84.
    • Liberman, I. Y., Mann, V. A., Shankweiler, D., & Werfelman, M. (1982). Children's memory for recurring linguistic and nonlinguistic material in relation to reading ability. Cortex, 18(3), 367-375.
    • MacDonald, S., (2009). Windows of reflection from Adults with Dyslexia: Conceptualising Dyslexia Using the Social Model of Disability. Dyslexia. An International Journal of Research and Practice, 15 (4). pp. 347-362.
    • Nicolson, R. I., & Fawcett, A. J. (1990). Automaticity: A new framework for dyslexia research?. Cognition, 35(2), 159-182.
    • O'Brien, C. P. (2008). The CAGE Questionnaire For Detection Of Alcoholism. JAMA: The Journal of the American Medical Association, 300(17), 2054-2056.
    • Parrott, A. C., Sisk, E., & Turner, J. J. D. (2000). Psychobiological Problems In Heavy 'Ecstasy' (MDMA) Polydrug Users. Drug And Alcohol Dependence, 60(1), 105-110.
    • Pavlidis, G. T. (1991). Diagnostic Significance And Relationship Between Dyslexia And Erratic Eye Movements. In J. F. Stein (Ed.), Vision And Visual Dyslexia (Pp. 263-270). London: Macmillan.
    • Pothos, E. M., & Cox, W. M. (2002). Cognitive Bias For Alcohol-Related Information In Inferential Processes. Drug And Alcohol Dependence, 66(3), 235-241.
    • Ray, N. J., Fowler, S., & Stein, J. F. (2005). Yellow filters can improve magnocellular function: motion sensitivity, convergence, accommodation, and reading. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1039(1), 283-293.
    • Roach, N. W., & Hogben, J. H. (2007). Impaired filtering of behaviourally irrelevant visual information in dyslexia. Brain, 130(3), 771-785.
    • Rodgers, J., Buchanan, T., Scholey, A. B., Heffernan, T. M., Ling, J., & Parrott, A. C. (2003). Patterns of drug use and the influence of gender on self-reports of memory ability in ecstasy users: a web-based study. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 17(4), 389-396.
    • Shaywitz, S. E. (1998). Current Concepts - Dyslexia. New England Journal Of Medicine, 338(5), 307-312.
    • Sparks, R., Patton, J., Ganschow, L., Humbach, N., & Javorsky, J. (2008). Early first-Language Reading And Spelling Skills Predict Later Second-Language Reading And Spelling Skills. Journal Of Educational Psychology, 100, 162-174.
    • Stacy, A. W. (1997). Memory Activation And Expectancy As Prospective Predictors Of Alcohol And Marijuana Use. Journal Of Abnormal Psychology, 106, 61-73.
    • Stanovich, K. E. (1988). Explaining the differences between the dyslexic and the gardenvariety poor reader The phonological-core variable-difference model.Journal of learning disabilities, 21(10), 590-604.
    • Stein, J. (1990). Unstable Binocular Control And Poor Visual Direction Sense In Developmental Dyslexics. In G. Hales (Ed.), Meeting Points In Dyslexia. Reading, England: British Dyslexia Association.
    • Steptoe, A., Wardle, J., Cui, W., Bellisle, F., Zotti, A. M., Baranyai, R., & Sanderman, R. (2002). Trends in smoking, diet, physical exercise, and attitudes toward health in European university students from 13 countries, 1990-2000. Preventive medicine, 35(2), 97-104.
    • Tallal, P. (1980). Auditory Temporal Perception, Phonics, And Reading Disabilities In Children. Brain And Language, 9(2), 182-198.
    • Tiffany, S. T. (1990). A Cognitive Model Of Drug Urges And Drug-Use Behavior: Role Of Automatic And Nonautomatic Processes. Psychological Review, 97, 147-168.
    • Vellutino, F. R., Fletcher, J. M., Snowling, M. J., & Scanlon, D. M. (2004). Specific reading disability (dyslexia): What have we learned in the past four decades?. Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, 45(1), 2-40.
    • Vinegrad, M. (1994) A Revised Adult Dyslexia Checklist. Educare No. 48, Pp. 21-23, March 1994.
    • Webb, E., Ashton, C. H., Kelly, P., & Kamali, F. (1996). Alcohol and drug use in UK university students. The lancet, 348(9032), 922-925.
    • Yates, R. (2012). Bad mouthing, bad habits and bad, bad, boys: an exploration of the relationship between dyslexia and drug dependence. Mental health and substance use, 6(3), 184-202.
  • No related research data.
  • Discovered through pilot similarity algorithms. Send us your feedback.

Share - Bookmark

Cite this article