LOGIN TO YOUR ACCOUNT

Username
Password
Remember Me
Or use your Academic/Social account:

CREATE AN ACCOUNT

Or use your Academic/Social account:

Congratulations!

You have just completed your registration at OpenAire.

Before you can login to the site, you will need to activate your account. An e-mail will be sent to you with the proper instructions.

Important!

Please note that this site is currently undergoing Beta testing.
Any new content you create is not guaranteed to be present to the final version of the site upon release.

Thank you for your patience,
OpenAire Dev Team.

Close This Message

CREATE AN ACCOUNT

Name:
Username:
Password:
Verify Password:
E-mail:
Verify E-mail:
*All Fields Are Required.
Please Verify You Are Human:
fbtwitterlinkedinvimeoflicker grey 14rssslideshare1
Rossetti, Yves; Holmes, Nicholas; Rode, Gilles; Farne, Alessandro (2010)
Publisher: Schattauer Verlag
Languages: English
Types: Part of book or chapter of book
Subjects:
Dualism has long distinguished between the mental and the body experiences. Probing the structure and organisation of the self traditionally calls for a distinction between these two sides of the self coin. It is far beyond the scope of this chapter to address these philosophical issues, and our starting point will be the simple distinction between reflective processes involved in the elaboration of body image, self awareness and self-recognition (i.e. ‘the self’) and the sensori-motor dialogues involved in action control, reactions and automatisms (i.e. ‘the body’ schema). This oversimplification does not take into account the complex interactions taking place between these two levels of description, but our initial aim will be to distinguish between them, before addressing the question of their interactions. Cognitive and sensori-motor processes have frequently been distinguished (review: Rossetti and Revonsuo 2000), and it may be proposed that a similar dissociation can be put forward, a priori, between a central representation of self and a bodily representation corresponding to body schema (Figure 1).
  • The results below are discovered through our pilot algorithms. Let us know how we are doing!

    • Bisiach, E., M.L. Rusconi & G. Vallar (1991). Remission of somatoparaphrenic delusion through vestibular stimulation. Neuropsychologia 29, 1029-1031.
    • Bisiach, E., M.L. Rusconi & G. Vallar (1991). Remission of somatoparaphrenic delusion through vestibular stimulation. Neuropsychologia 29, 1029-1031.
    • Calabria, M., C. Michel, J. Honoré, L. Pisella, G. Rode, D. Boisson & Y. Rossetti (2004). Poster presented at the first European Neuropsychology meeting, Modena, april 2004.
    • Cappa, S., R. Sterzi, G. Vallar & E. Bisiach (1987). Remission of hemineglect and anosognosia during vestibular stimulation. Neuropsychologia, 25, 775-782.
    • Cappa, S., R. Sterzi, G. Vallar & E. Bisiach (1987). Remission of hemineglect and anosognosia during vestibular stimulation. Neuropsychologia, 25, 775-782.
    • Daprati E, Franck N, Georgieff N, Proust J, Pacherie E, Dalery J, Jeannerod M. (1997) Looking for the agent: an investigation into consciousness of action and selfconsciousness in schizophrenic patients. Cognition 65(1):71-86.
    • Dolto, F. (1984). L'image inconsciente du corps. Paris: Seuil.
    • Driver, J. & J.B. Mattingly (1998). Parietal neglect and visual awareness. Nat. Neurosci. 1(1), 17-22.
    • Farnè, A., Y. Rossetti, S. Toniolo & E. Làdavas (2002). Ameliorating neglect with prism adaptation: Visuo-manual vs. visuo-verbal measures. Neuropsychologia, 40/7, 1069- 1080.
    • Farnè, A., Y. Rossetti, S. Toniolo & E. Làdavas (2002). Ameliorating neglect with prism adaptation: Visuo-manual vs. visuo-verbal measures. Neuropsychologia, 40/7, 1069- 1080.
    • Farrer C, Franck N, Paillard J, Jeannerod M. (2003) The role of proprioception in action recognition. Conscious Cogn. 12(4):609-19.
    • Felician O, Ceccaldi M, Didic M, Thinus-Blanc C, Poncet M. (2003) Pointing to body parts: a double dissociation study. Neuropsychologia;41(10):1307-16.
    • Frassinetti F, Angeli V, Meneghello F, Avanzi S, Làdavas E. (2002) Long-lasting amelioration of visuospatial neglect by prism adaptation. Brain. 125:608-23.
    • Frassinetti F, Maini M, Romualdi S, Galante E, Avanzi S. (2008) Is it mine? Hemispheric asymmetries in corporeal self-recognition. J Cogn Neurosci.;20(8):1507-16.
    • Gallagher, S. (1986). Body image and body schema: a conceptual clarification. Journal of Dijkerman, H. C., R.D. McIntosh, Y. Rossetti, C. Tilikete, R.C. Roberts & A.D. Milner (2003). Ocular Scanning and Perceptual Size Distortion in Hemispatial Neglect: Effects of Prism Adaptation and Sequential Stimulus Presentation. Experimental Brain Research, 153(2), 220-30.
    • Head, H. & G. Holmes (1911). Sensory disturbances from brain lesions. Brain 34, 102-154.
    • Jacquin-Courtois S, Rode G, Pisella L, Boisson D, Rossetti Y. (2008) Wheel-chair driving improvement following visuo-manual prism adaptation. Cortex 44(1):90-6.
    • Lhermitte, J. (1942). De l'image corporelle. Revue Neurologique, 74, 20-38.
    • Maravita A, McNeil J, Malhotra P, Greenwood R, Husain M, Driver J. (2003) Prism adaptation can improve contralesional tactile perception in neglect. Neurology. 10;60(11):1829-31.
    • McIntosh, R.M., Y. Rossetti & A.D. Milner (2002). Prism adaptation improves chronic visual and haptic neglect. Cortex, 38, 309-320.
    • McIntosh, R.M., Y. Rossetti & A.D. Milner (2002). Prism adaptation improves chronic visual and haptic neglect. Cortex, 38, 309-320.
    • Michel C. (2006) Simulating unilateral neglect in normals: myth or reality? Restor Neurol Neurosci. 24(4-6):419-30.
    • Paillard J. (1971) Les déterminants moteurs de l'organisation de l'espace. Cahiers de Psychologie, 14, N° 4, 261-316
    • Paillard J. (1980) Le corps situé et le corps identifié : Une approche psychophysiologique de la notion de schéma corporel. Rev. Méd. Suisse Romande, 100: 129.141, 1980
    • Paillard J. (1982) Le corps et ses langages d'espace : Nouvelles contributions psychophysiologiques à l'étude du schéma corporel. In E. Jeddi (ed.). Le corps en Psychiatrie (p. 53-69) Paris. Masson.
    • Paillard J. (1985) Les niveaux sensori-moteurs et cognitif du contrôle de l'action. In M. Laurent & P.Therme (eds). Recherches en Activités Physiques et Sportives, Publication du Centre de Recherches de l'UEREPS, Université Aix-Marseille II. pp. 147-163.
    • Paillard J., Michel F, Stelmach G (1983) Localization Without Content. A Tactile Analogue of 'Blind Sight'. Archives of Neurology, 40, 548-551.
    • Paillard, J. (1999). Body schema and body image - a double dissociation in deafferented patients. In G.N. Gantchev, S. Mori & J. Massion (Eds.), Motor control, today and tomorrow.
    • Paillard, J. (2005) Vectorial versus configural encoding of Body Space A neural basis for a distinction between Body schema and Body image. In : V.Knockaert & H. De Preester (eds) Body Image and Body Schema: Interdisciplinary perspectives. (pp. 89-109). John Benjamin, Amsterdam
    • Pisella, L. & Y. Rossetti (2000). Interaction between conscious identification and nonconscious sensori-motor processing: temporal constraints. In Y. Rossetti & A. Revonsuo (Eds.), Beyond dissociation: Interaction between dissociated implicit and explicit processing (129-151). Amsterdam: Benjamins.
    • Redding GM, Rossetti Y, Wallace B. (2005) Applications of prism adaptation: a tutorial in theory and method. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 29(3):431-44.
    • Rode, G., C. Tilikete, J. Luauté, Y. Rossetti, A. Vighetto & D. Boission (2002). Bilateral vestibular stimulation does not improve hemineglect. Neurospychologia 40, 1104-1106.
    • Rode, G., L. Pisella, Y. Rossetti, A. Farnè & D. Boisson (2003). Bottom-up transfer of visuomotor plasticity. Prism adaptation. Progress in Brain Research series vol.142, (273- 287). Elsevier Amsterdam.
    • Rode, G., M.T. Perenin, J. Honoré, D. Boisson (1998). Improvement of the motor deficit of neglect patients through vestibular stimulation: evidence for a motor neglect component. Cortex, 34, 253-261.
    • Rode, G., M.T. Perenin, J. Honoré, D. Boisson (1998). Improvement of the motor deficit of neglect patients through vestibular stimulation: evidence for a motor neglect component. Cortex, 34, 253-261.
    • Rode, G., Y. Rossetti & D. Boisson (2001). Prism adaptation improves representational neglect. Neuropsychologia, 39(11), 1250-4.
    • Rode, G., Y. Rossetti, L. Li & D. Boisson (1998). The effect of prism adaptation on neglect for visual imagery. Behavioural Neurology, 11, 251-258.
    • Rossetti, Y. & G. Rode (2002). Reducing spatial neglect by visual and other sensory manipulations: non-cognitive (physiological) routes to the rehabilitation of a cognitive disorder. In H.O. Karnathh, A.D. Milner, G. Vallar (Eds.), The cognitive and neural bases of spatial neglect (375-396). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
    • Rossetti, Y. & L. Pisella (2002). Tutorial: Several “vision for action” systems: A guide to dissociating and integrating dorsal and ventral functions. In W. Prinz & B. Hommel (Eds.), Attention and Performance XIX; Common mechanisms in perception and action (62-119). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
    • Rossetti, Y. (1998). Implicit short-lived motor representation of space in brain-damaged and healthy subjects. Consciousness and Cognition, 7, 520-558.
    • Rossetti, Y. (2001). Implicit perception in action: short-lived motor representations of space. In P.G. Grossenbacher (Ed), Finding consciousness in the brain (131-179). Amsterdam: Benjamins.
    • Rossetti, Y., G. Rode & D. Boisson (2001). Numbsense : a case study and implications. In B. De Gelder, E. De Haan & C. Heywood (Eds.), Out of mind: Varieties of unconscious processing (265-292). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
    • Rossetti, Y., G. Rode, L. Pisella, A. Farné, L. Li L., D. Boisson & M.-T. Perenin (1998). Prism adaptation to a rightward optical deviation rehabilitates left hemispatial neglect. Nature, 395, 166-169.
    • Rossetti, Y., G. Rode, L. Pisella, A. Farne, L. Ling & D. Boisson (1999). Sensorimotor plasticity and cognition: prism adaptation can affect various levels of space representation. In M. Grealy J.A. Thomson (Eds.), Studies in Perception and Action, (265-269). Mahwah, NJ, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
    • Tilikete, C., G. Rode, Y. Rossetti, L. Li, J. Pichon & D. Boisson (2001). Prism adaptation to rightward optical deviation improves postural imbalance in left hemiparetic patients. Current Biology, 11, 524-528.
    • Vallar, G., G. Bottini & R. Sterzi (2003). Anosognosia for left-sided motor and sensory deficits, motor neglect, and sensory hemiinattention: is there a relationship? In C. Prablanc, D. Pelisson & Y. Rossetti (Eds.), Progress in Brain Research, Vol. 142. (199- 310). Amsterdam, Elsevier Science B.V.
    • van den Bos E, Jeannerod M. (2002) Sense of body and sense of action both contribute to self-recognition. Cognition.85(2):177-87.
  • No related research data.
  • No similar publications.

Share - Bookmark

Cite this article