LOGIN TO YOUR ACCOUNT

Username
Password
Remember Me
Or use your Academic/Social account:

CREATE AN ACCOUNT

Or use your Academic/Social account:

Congratulations!

You have just completed your registration at OpenAire.

Before you can login to the site, you will need to activate your account. An e-mail will be sent to you with the proper instructions.

Important!

Please note that this site is currently undergoing Beta testing.
Any new content you create is not guaranteed to be present to the final version of the site upon release.

Thank you for your patience,
OpenAire Dev Team.

Close This Message

CREATE AN ACCOUNT

Name:
Username:
Password:
Verify Password:
E-mail:
Verify E-mail:
*All Fields Are Required.
Please Verify You Are Human:
fbtwitterlinkedinvimeoflicker grey 14rssslideshare1
Reynolds, Gemma; Field, Andy P.; Askew, Chris (2016)
Publisher: Taylor & Francis
Languages: English
Types: Article
Subjects: B, psychology
Recent research has indicated that vicarious learning can lead to increases in children’s fear beliefs and avoidance preferences for stimuli and that these fear responses can subsequently be reversed using positive modeling (counterconditioning). The current study investigated children’s vicariously acquired avoidance behavior, physiological responses (heart rate), and attentional bias for stimuli and whether these could also be reduced via counterconditioning. Ninety-six (49 boys, 47 girls) 7- to 11-year-olds received vicarious fear learning for novel stimuli and were then randomly assigned to a counterconditioning, extinction, or control group. Fear beliefs and avoidance preferences were measured pre- and post-learning, whereas avoidance behavior, heart rate, and attentional bias were all measured post-learning. Control group children showed increases in fear beliefs and avoidance preferences for animals seen in vicarious fear learning trials. In addition, significantly greater avoidance behavior, heart rate responding, and attentional bias were observed for these animals compared to a control animal. In contrast, vicariously acquired avoidance preferences of children in the counterconditioning group were significantly reduced post-positive modeling, and these children also did not show the heightened heart rate responding to fear-paired animals. Children in the extinction group demonstrated comparable responses to the control group; thus the extinction procedure showed no effect on any fear measures. The findings suggest that counterconditioning with positive modelling can be used as an effective early intervention to reduce the behavioral and physiological effects of vicarious fear learning in childhood.
  • The results below are discovered through our pilot algorithms. Let us know how we are doing!

    • Askew, C., Dunne, G., Özdil, Z., Reynolds, G., & Field, A. P. (2013). Stimulus fear-relevance and the vicarious learning pathway to childhood fears. Emotion, 13, 915-925. doi:10.1037/a0032714
    • Askew, C., & Field, A. P. (2007). Vicarious learning and the development of fears in childhood. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45, 2616-2627. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2007.06.008
    • Askew, C., & Field, A. P. (2008). The vicarious learning pathway to fear 40 years on. Clinical Psychology Review, 28, 1249-1265. doi:10.1016/j. cpr.2008.05.003
    • Askew, C., Kessock-Philip, H., & Field, A. P. (2008). Interactions between the indirect pathways to fear in children: What happens when verbal threat information and vicarious learning combine? Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 36, 491-505. doi:10.1017/ S1352465808004402
    • Askew, C., Reynolds, G., Fielding-Smith, S., & Field, A. P. (2016). Inhibition of vicariously learned fear in children using positive modeling and prior exposure. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 125, 279-291.
    • Bandura, A., & Rosenthal, T. L. (1966). Vicarious classical conditioning as a function of arousal level. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 3, 54-62. doi:10.1037/h0022639
    • Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B. M., & Walker, S. (2014). lme4: Linear mixed-effects models using Eigen and S4. (R package, Version 1.1-7). Retrieved from http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=lme4
    • Berger, S. M. (1962). Conditioning through vicarious instigation. Psychological Review, 69(5), 450-466. doi:10.1037/h0046466
    • Bouton, M. E. (1993). Context, time, and memory retrieval in the interference paradigms of pavlovian learning. Psychological Bulletin, 114(1), 80-99. doi:10.1037/00332909.114.1.80
    • Bouton, M. E. (2002). Context, ambiguity, and unlearning: Sources of relapse after behavioral extinction. Biological Psychiatry, 52(10), 976- 986. doi:10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01546-9
    • Bouton, M. E. (2004). Context and behavioral processes in extinction. Learning & Memory, 11, 485-494. doi:10.1101/lm.78804
    • Bouton, M. E., Mineka, S., & Barlow, D. H. (2001). A modern learning theory perspective on the etiology of panic disorder. Psychological Review, 108, 4-32. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.108.1.4
    • Bradley, B. P., Mogg, K., White, J., Groom, C., & De Bono, J. (1999). Attentional bias for emotional faces in generalized anxiety disorder. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 38, 267-278. doi:10.1348/ 014466599162845
    • Broeren, S., Lester, K. J., Muris, P., & Field, A. P. (2011). They are afraid of the animal, so therefore I am too: Influence of peer modeling on fear beliefs and approach-avoidance behaviors towards animals in typically developing children. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 49, 50-57. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2010.11.001
    • Brown, H. M., Eley, T. C., Broeren, S., MacLeod, C., Rinck, M., Hadwin, J. A., & Lester, K. J. (2014). Psychometric properties of reaction time based experimental paradigms measuring anxiety-related informationprocessing biases in children. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 28, 97- 107. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2013.11.004
    • Cisler, J. M., & Koster, E. H. W. (2010). Mechanisms of attentional biases toward threat in anxiety disorders: An integrative review. Clinical Psychology Review, 30, 203-216. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2009.11.003
    • Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Academic Press.
    • Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 155-159. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.155
    • Craske, M. G., Kircanski, K., Zelikowsky, M., Mystkowski, J., Chowdhury, N., & Baker, A. (2008). Optimizing inhibitory learning during exposure therapy. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 46, 5-27. doi:10.1016/j. brat.2007.10.003
    • Davey, G. C. L. (1997). A conditioning model of phobias. In G. C. L. Davey (Ed.), Phobias: A handbook of theory, research, and treatment (pp. 301-322). Chichester, UK: Wiley.
    • Dubi, K., Rapee, R. M., Emerton, J., & Schniering, C. (2008). Maternal modeling and the acquisition of fear and avoidance in toddlers: Influence of stimulus preparedness and child temperament. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 36, 499-512. doi:10.1007/s10802-007-9195-3
    • Dunn, L. M., Dunn, D. M., Styles, B., & Sewell, J. (2009). The British Picture Vocabulary Scale III (3rd ed.). London, UK: GL Assessment.
    • Dunne, G., & Askew, C. (2013). Vicarious learning and unlearning of fear in childhood via mother and stranger models. Emotion, 13, 974-980. doi:10.1037/a0032994
    • Dunne, G., & Askew, C. (2015). Fear relevance and the speed and robustness of vicarious fear learning in Children. Manuscript submitted for publication.
    • Egliston, K.-A., & Rapee, R. M. (2007). Inhibition of fear acquisition in toddlers following positive modelling by their mothers. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45, 1871-1882. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2007.02.007
    • Field, A. P. (2006a). Is conditioning a useful framework for understanding the development and treatment of phobias? Clinical Psychology Review, 26, 857-875. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2005.05.010
    • Field, A. P. (2006b). Watch out for the beast: Fear information and attentional bias in children. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 35, 431-439. doi:10.1207/s15374424jccp3503_8
    • Field, A. P., Hadwin, J. A., & Lester, K. J. (2011). Information processing biases in child and adolescent anxiety: Evidence and origins. In W. K. Silverman & A. P. Field (Eds.), Anxiety disorders in children and adolescents: Research, assessment and intervention (2nd ed., pp. 103-128). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
    • Field, A. P., & Lawson, J. (2003). Fear information and the development of fears during childhood: Effects on implicit fear responses and behavioural avoidance. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 41, 1277-1293. doi:10.1016/S0005-7967(03)00034-2
    • Field, A. P., & Purkis, H. M. (2011). The role of learning in the aetiology of child and adolescent fear and anxiety. In W. K. Silverman & A. P. Field (Eds.), Anxiety disorders in children and adolescents: Research, assessment and intervention (2nd ed., pp. 227-256). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
    • Field, A. P., & Storksen-Coulson, H. (2007). The interaction of pathways to fear in childhood anxiety: A preliminary study. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45, 3051-3059. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2007.09.001
    • Garner, M. (2010). Assessment of attentional bias using the dot-probe task in anxious children and adolescents. In J. Hadwin & A. P. Field (Eds.), Information processing biases and anxiety: A developmental perspective. Chichester, UK: Wiley-Blackwell.
    • Gerull, F. C., & Rapee, R. M. (2002). Mother knows best: Effects of maternal modelling on the acquisition of fear and avoidance behaviour in toddlers. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 40, 279-287. doi:10.1016/ S0005-7967(01)00013-4
    • Golkar, A., Selbing, I., Flygare, O., Öhman, A., & Olsson, A. (2013). Other people as means to a safe end: Vicarious extinction blocks the return of learned fear. Psychological Science, 24, 2182-2190. doi:10.1177/ 0956797613489890
    • Gullone, E., & King, N. (1997). Three-year follow-up of normal fear in children and adolescents aged 7 to 18 years. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 15, 97-111. doi:10.1111/bjdp.1997.15. issue-1
    • Hadwin, J. A., & Field, A. P. (Eds.). (2010). Information processing biases and anxiety: A developmental perspective. Chichester, UK: WileyBlackwell. doi:10.1002/9780470661468
    • Hodgson, R., & Rachman, S. (1974). Desynchrony in measures of fear. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 12, 319-326. doi:10.1016/0005-7967 (74)90006-0
    • Hygge, S., & Öhman, A. (1978). Modeling processes in the acquisition of fears: Vicarious electrodermal conditioning to fear-relevant stimuli. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 271-279. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.36.3.271
    • Kelly, V. L., Barker, H., Field, A. P., Wilson, C., & Reynolds, S. (2010). Can Rachman's indirect pathways be used to un-learn fear? A prospective paradigm to test whether children's fears can be reduced using positive information and modelling a non-anxious response. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 48, 164-170. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2009.10.002
    • Kravetz, D. F. (1974). Heart rate as a minimal cue for the occurrence of vicarious classical conditioning. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 29, 125-131. doi:10.1037/h0035679
    • Lang, P. J. (1968). Fear reduction and fear behavior: Problems in treating a construct. In J. M. Schlien (Ed.), Research in psychotherapy (Vol. 3, pp. 90-103). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. doi:10.1037/10546-004
    • MacLeod, C., Mathews, A., & Tata, P. (1986). Attentional bias in emotional disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 95, 15-20. doi:10.1037/ 0021-843X.95.1.15
    • Mathews, A., & MacLeod, C. (2002). Induced processing biases have causal effects on anxiety. Cognition & Emotion, 16, 310-315. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.36.3.271
    • McNeil, D. W., Vrana, S. R., Melamed, B. G., Cuthbert, B. N., & Lang, P. J. (1993). Emotional imagery in simple and social phobia: Fear versus anxiety. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 102, 212-225. doi:10.1037/ 0021-843X.102.2.212
    • Merckelbach, H., De Jong, P. J., Muris, P., & Van den Hout, M. A. (1996). The etiology of specific phobias: A review. Clinical Psychology Review, 16, 337-361. doi:10.1016/0272-7358(96)00014-1
    • Merckelbach, H., De Ruiter, C., Van den Hout, M. A., & Hoekstra, R. (1989). Conditioning experiences and phobias. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 27, 657-662. doi:10.1016/0005-7967(89)90149-6
    • Milad, M. R., Orr, S. P., Pitman, R. K., & Rauch, S. L. (2005). Context modulation of memory for fear extinction in humans. Psychophysiology, 42, 456-464. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2005.00302.x
    • Milad, M. R., & Quirk, G. J. (2012). Fear extinction as a model for translational neuroscience: Ten years of progress. Annual Review of Psychology, 63, 129-151. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.121208.131631
    • Mineka, S. (1985). Animal models of anxiety-based disorders: Their usefulness and limitations. In J. Maser & A. Tuma (Eds.), Anxiety and the anxiety disorders (pp. 199-244). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
    • Mineka, S., & Cook, M. (1993). Mechanisms involved in the observational conditioning of fear. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122, 23-38. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.122.1.23
    • Mineka, S., & Zinbarg, R. (2006). A contemporary learning theory perspective on the etiology of anxiety disorders: It's not what you thought it was. American Psychologist, 61, 10-26. doi:10.1037/0003-066x.61.1.10
    • Mogg, K., & Bradley, B. P. (1998). A cognitive-motivational analysis of anxiety. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 36, 809-848. doi:10.1016/ S0005-7967(98)00063-1
    • Mogg, K., Bradley, B. P., & Williams, R. (1995). Attentional bias in anxiety and depression: The role of awareness. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 34, 17−36. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8260.1995.tb01434.x
    • Muris, P., Merckelbach, H., De Jong, P. J., & Ollendick, T. H. (2002). The etiology of specific fears and phobias in children: A critique of the nonassociative account. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 40, 185-195. doi:10.1016/S0005-7967(01)00051-1
    • Myers, K. M., & Davis, M. (2007). Mechanisms of fear extinction. Molecular Psychiatry, 12, 120-150. doi:10.1038/sj.mp.4001939
    • Ollendick, T. H., & King, N. J. (1991). Origins of childhood fears: An evaluation of Rachman's theory of fear acquisition. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 29, 117-123. doi:10.1016/0005-7967(91)90039-6
    • Öst, L.-G. (1985). Ways of acquiring phobias and outcome of behavioral treatments. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 23, 683-689. doi:10.1016/ 0005-7967(85)90066-X
    • Öst, L.-G. (1987). Age of onset in different phobias. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 96, 223-229. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.96.3.223
    • Öst, L.-G., & Hugdahl, K. (1981). Acquisition of phobias and anxiety response patterns in clinical patients. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 19, 439-447. doi:10.1016/0005-7967(81)90134-0
    • Prenoveau, J. M., Craske, M. G., Liao, B., & Ornitz, E. M. (2012). Human fear conditioning and extinction: Timing is everything…or is it? Biological Psychology, 92, 59-68. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.02.005
    • Rachman, S. (1977). The conditioning theory of fear-acquisition: A critical examination. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 15, 375-387. doi:10.1016/0005-7967(77)90041-9
    • Ratcliff, R. (1993). Methods for dealing with reaction time outliers. Psychological Bulletin, 114, 510-532. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.114.3.510
    • Rescorla, R. A., & Wagner, A. R. (1972). A theory of pavlovian conditioning: Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement. In A. H. Black & W. F. Prokasy (Eds.), Classical conditioning II: Current research and theory (pp. 64-99). New York, NY: AppletonCentury-Crofts.
    • Reynolds, G., Field, A. P., & Askew, C. (2014). The effect of vicarious fear learning on children's heart rate responses and attentional bias for novel animals. Emotion, 14, 995-1006. doi:10.1037/a0037225
    • Reynolds, G., Field, A. P., & Askew, C. (2015). Preventing the development of observationally learnt fears in children by devaluing the model's negative response. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 43, 1355- 1367. doi:10.1007/s10802-015-0004-0
    • Sartory, G., Rachman, S., & Grey, S. (1977). An investigation of the relation between reported fear and heart rate. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 15, 435-438. doi:10.1016/0005-7967(77)90048-1
    • Tottenham, N., Tanaka, J. W., Leon, A. C., McCarry, T., Nurse, M., Hare, T. A., … Nelson, C. (2009). The NimStim set of facial expressions: Judgments from untrained research participants. Psychiatry Research, 168, 242-249. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2008.05.006
    • Weems, C. F., Zakem, A. H., Costa, N. M., Cannon, M. F., & Watts, S. E. (2005). Physiological response and childhood anxiety: Association with symptoms of anxiety disorders and cognitive bias. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 34, 712-723. doi:10.1207/ s15374424jccp3404_13
    • Zinbarg, R. E. (1998). Concordance and synchrony in measures of anxiety and panic reconsidered: A hierarchical model of anxiety and panic. Behavior Therapy, 29, 301-323. doi:10.1016/S0005-7894(98)80009-9
  • No related research data.
  • Discovered through pilot similarity algorithms. Send us your feedback.

Share - Bookmark

Funded by projects

Cite this article