LOGIN TO YOUR ACCOUNT

Username
Password
Remember Me
Or use your Academic/Social account:

CREATE AN ACCOUNT

Or use your Academic/Social account:

Congratulations!

You have just completed your registration at OpenAire.

Before you can login to the site, you will need to activate your account. An e-mail will be sent to you with the proper instructions.

Important!

Please note that this site is currently undergoing Beta testing.
Any new content you create is not guaranteed to be present to the final version of the site upon release.

Thank you for your patience,
OpenAire Dev Team.

Close This Message

CREATE AN ACCOUNT

Name:
Username:
Password:
Verify Password:
E-mail:
Verify E-mail:
*All Fields Are Required.
Please Verify You Are Human:
fbtwitterlinkedinvimeoflicker grey 14rssslideshare1
De Cat, C; Gusnanto, A; Serratrice, L
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Languages: English
Types: Article
Subjects:
The literature exploring the executive function correlates of bilingualism is vast, but to date, few studies have concentrated on children, for whom the bilingual advantage appears even more inconsistent than for adults. We investigate a highly heterogeneous group of children (in terms of bilingual experience and socio-economic status) and identify the critical threshold of bilingual experience from which an advantage can be observed at group level. The modeling methods adopted allow the use of fine-grained, continuous factors for age and socio-economic status, thereby effectively controlling for their effect and isolating the specific effect of bilingual experience. We pioneer the use of Cox Proportional Hazard regression to analyze performance in the Simon task. This allows the modeling of all data points without transformation nor outlier removal, and captures both accuracy and reaction time within the same analysis, while also being able to handle multiple predictor variables.
  • The results below are discovered through our pilot algorithms. Let us know how we are doing!

    • Abutalebi, J., Della Rosa, P. A., Green, D. W., Hernandez, M., Scifo, P., Keim, R., . . . Costa, A. (2011). Bilingualism tunes the anterior cingulate cortex for conflict monitoring. Cerebral Cortex, 9 , 2076-86.
    • Abutalebi, J., & Rietbergen, M. J. (2014). Neuroplasticity of the bilingual brain: Cognitive control and reserve. Applied Psycholinguistics, 35 , 895-899.
    • Adesope, O. O., Lavin, T., Thompson, T., & Ungerleider, C. (2010). A systematic review and meta-analysis of the cognitive correlates of bilingualism. Review of Educational Research, 80 , 207-245.
    • Antón, E., Duñabeitia, J. A., Estévez, A., Hernández, J. A., Castillo, A., Fuentes, L. J., . . . Carreiras, M. (2014). Is there a bilingual advantage in the ANT task? Evidence from children. Frontiers in Psychology, 5 , 00398.
    • Ardila, A., Rosselli, M., Matute, E., & Guajardo, S. (2005). The influence of the parents' educational level on the development of executive functions. Developmental Neuropsychology, 28 , 539-560.
    • Armitage, P., Berry, G., & Matthews, J. N. (2008). Statistical methods in medical research (Fourth ed.). Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell.
    • Baddeley, A. (2000). The episodic buffer: A new component of working memory? Trends Cognitive Science, 4 , 417-423.
    • Baum, S., & Titone, D. (2014). Moving toward a neuroplasticity view of bilingualism, executive control, and aging. Applied Psycholinguistics, 35 , 857-894.
    • Bedore, L., Peña, E., Summers, C., Boerger, K., Resendiz, M., Greene, K., . . . Gillam, R. (2012). The measure matters: Language dominance profiles across measures in Spanish-English bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15 , 616-629.
    • Best, J. R. (2012). Exergaming immediately enhances children's executive function. Developmental Psychology, 48 , 1501-1510.
    • Bialystok, E. (1999). Cognitive complexity and attentional control in the bilingual mind. Child Development, Volume 70 , 636-644.
    • Bialystok, E., Craik, F., Klein, R., & Viswanathan, M. (2004). Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: evidence from the Simon task. Psychology and Aging, 19 , .
    • Bialystok, E., & Martin, M. (2004). Attention and inhibition in bilingual children: Evidence from the Dimensional Change Card Sort task. Developmental Science, 7 , 325-339.
    • Bialystok, E., Martin, M., & Viswanathan, M. (2005). Bilingualism across the lifespan: The rise and fall of inhibitory control. International Journal of Bilingualism, 9 , 103-119.
    • Blom, E., Küntay, A. C., Messer, M., Verhagen, J., & Leseman, P. (2014). The benefits of being bilingual: Working memory in bilingual Turkish-Dutch children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 128 , 105-119.
    • Brito, N., Sebastián-Gallés, N., & Barr, R. (2015). Differences in language exposure and its effects on memory flexibility in monolingual, bilingual, and trilingual infants. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18 , 670-682.
    • Brocki, K., & Bohlin, G. (2004). Executive functions in children aged 6 to 13: A dimensional and developmental study. Developmental Neuropsychology, 26 , 571-93.
    • Bryan, J., & Luszcz, M. A. (2000). Measurement of executive function: Considerations for detecting adult age differences. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 22 , 40-55.
    • Buckner, R. L. (2004). Memory and executive function in aging and AD: Multiple factors that cause decline and reserve factors that compensate. Neuron, 44 , 195-208.
    • Calvo, A., & Bialystok, E. (2014). Independent effects of bilingualism and socioeconomic status on language ability and executive functioning. Cognition, 130 , 278-288.
    • Carlson, S., & Meltzoff, A. (2008). Bilingual experience and executive functioning in young children. Developmental Science, 11 , 282-298.
    • Collett, D. (2015). Modelling survival data in medical research (Third edition ed.). Bristol: CRC press.
    • de Bruin, A., Treccani, B., & Della Sala, S. (2015). Cognitive advantage in bilingualism an example of publication bias? Psychological Science, 26 , 99-107.
    • Dempster, F. N. (1992). The rise and fall of the inhibitory mechanism: Toward a unified theory of cognitive development and aging. Developmental Review, 12 , 45-75.
    • Diamond, A. (2011). Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64 , 135-168.
    • Diamond, A., & Lee, K. (2011). Interventions shown to aid executive function development in children 4 to 12 years old. Science, 333 , 959-964.
    • Donnelly, S., Brooks, P. J., & Homer, B. (2015). Examining the bilingual advantage on conflict resolution tasks: A meta-analysis. In D. C. Noelle et al. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 37th Annual Conference of the cognitive Science Society. Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.
    • Duñabeitia, J. A., Hernández, J. A., Antón, E., Macizo, P., Estévez, A., Fuentes, L. J., & Carreiras, M. (2015). The inhibitory advantage in bilingual children revisited. Journal of Experimental psychology, 61 , 234-251.
    • Engel De Abreu, P. (2011). Working memory in multilingual children: Is there a bilingual effect? Memory, 19 , 529-537.
    • Engel de Abreu, P., Baldassi, M., Puglisi, M., & Befi-Lopes, D. (2013). Cross-linguistic and crosscultural effects on verbal working memory and vocabulary: Testing language-minority children with an immigrant background. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Disorders, 56 , 630- 42.
    • Engel de Abreu, P., Cruz-Santos, A., Tourinho, C. J., Martin, R., & Bialystok, E. (2012). Bilingualism enriches the poor : Enhanced cognitive control in low-income minority children. Psychological Science, 23 , 1364-1371.
    • Eriksen, B. A., & Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Perception and psychophysics, 16 , 143-149.
    • Espy, K. A., & Bull, R. (2005). Inhibitory processes in young children and individual variation in short-term memory. Developmental Neuropsychology, 28 , 669-688.
    • Friedman, N., Miyake, A., Corley, R., Young, S., DeFries, J., & Hewitt, J. (2006). Not all executive functions are related to intelligence. Psychological Science, 17 , 172-179.
    • Garon, N., Bryson, S., & Smith, I. (2008). Executive function in preschoolers: A review using an integrative framework. Psychological Bulletin, 134 , 31-60.
    • Genesee, F. (2014). Commentary on "Moving toward a neuroplasticity view of bilingualism": The early years. Applied Psycholinguistics, 35 , 905-909.
    • Green, D. W. (1998). Mental control of the bilingual lexico-semantic system. Bilingualism: Language and cognition, 1 , 67-81.
    • Green, D. W. (2014). Individual variability and neuroplastic changes. Applied Psycholinguistics, 35 , 910-912.
    • Green, D. W., & Abutalebi, J. (2013). Language control in bilinguals: The adaptive control hypothesis. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25 , 515-530.
    • Gutiérrez-Clellen, V., Calderón, J., & Ellis Weismer, S. (2004). Verbal working memory in bilingual children. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Disorders, 47 , 863-876.
    • Hilchey, M., & Klein, R. (2011). Are there bilingual advantages on nonlinguistic interference tasks? Implications for the plasticity of executive control processes. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 18 , 625-658.
    • Hongwanishkul, D., Happaney, K. R., Lee, W., & Zelazo, P. D. (2005). Assessment of hot and cool executive function: Age-related changes and individual differences. Developmental Neuropsychology, 28 , 617-644.
    • Hougaard, P. (2000). Analysis of multivariate survival data. New York: Springer.
    • Iluz-Cohen, P., & Armon-Lotem, S. (2013). Language proficiency and executive control in bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 16 , 884-899. (10.1017/S1366728912000788) Ishihara, S. (1994). The series of plates designed as a test for colour-blindness. Tokyo: Kanehara and Co LTD.
    • Kaushanskaya, M., & Prior, A. (2015). Variability in the effects of bilingualism on cognition: It is not just about cognition, it is also about bilingualism. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18 , 27-28.
    • Kroll, J. F., Dussias, P. E., Bogulski, C. A., & Valdes Kroff, J. R. (2012). Juggling two languages in one mind: What bilinguals tell us about language processing and its consequences for cognition. In B. Ross (Ed.), Psychology of Learning and Motivation (Vol. 56, p. 229-262). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
    • Kroll, J. F., & Fricke, M. (2014). What bilinguals do with language that changes their minds and their brains. Applied Psycholinguistics, 35 , 921-925.
    • Lee, Y., Nelder, J. A., & Pawitan, Y. (2006). Generalized linear models with random effects: unified analysis via h-likelihood. London: CRC Press.
    • Los, S. A. (1996). On the origin of mixing costs: Exploring information processing in pure and mixed blocks of trials. Acta Psychologica, 94 , 145-188.
    • Luk, G. (2015). Who are the bilinguals (and monolinguals)? Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18 , 35-36.
    • Luk, G., Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., & Grady, C. L. (2011). Lifelong bilingualism maintains white matter integrity in older adults. The Journal of Neuroscience, 31 , 16808-16813.
    • Marinis, T., & Armon-Lotem, S. (2015). Sentence repetition. In S. Armon-Lotem, J. de Jong, & N. Meir (Eds.), Assessing multilingual children. Disentangling bilingualism from language impairment (p. 95-122). Bristol: Multilingual Matters.
    • Marinis, T., Chiat, S., Armon-Lotem, S., Gibbons, D., & Gipps, E. (2010). School-Age Sentence Imitation Test (SASIT). Reading: University of Reading.
    • Martin-Rhee, M., & Bialystok, E. (2008). The development of two types of inhibitory control in monolingual and bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 11 , 81-93.
    • Merz, E. C., Landry, S. H., JeffreyM.Williams, Barnes, M. A., Eisenberg, N., Spinrad, T. L., . . . School Readiness Research Consortium (2015). Parenting predictors of cognitive skills and emotion knowledge in socioeconomically disadvantaged preschoolers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 132C , 14-31.
    • Meuter, R. F. I., & Allport, A. (1999). Bilingual language switching in naming: Asymmetrical costs of language selection. Journal of memory and language, 40 , 25-40.
    • Mezzacappa, E. (2004). Alerting, orienting, and executive attention: Developmental properties and socio-demographic correlates in an epidemiological sample of young, urban children. Child Development, 75 , 1-14.
    • Miyake, A., Friedman, N., Emerson, M., Witzki, A., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex "frontal lobe" tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41 , 49-100.
    • Miyake, A., & Friedman, N. P. (2012). The nature and organization of individual differences in executive functions: Four general conclusions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21 , 8-14.
    • Monsell, S. (2003). Task switching. Trends in cognitive sciences, 7 , 134-140.
    • Morales, J., Calvo, A., & Bialystok, E. (2013). Working memory development in monolingual and bilingual children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 114 , 187-202.
    • Moreno, S., Bialystok, E., Barac, R., Schellenberg, E. G., Cepeda, N. J., & Chau, T. (2011). Short-term music training enhances verbal intelligence and executive function. Psychological Science, 22 , 1425-1433.
    • Morton, J. B., & Harper, S. N. (2007). What did Simon say? Revisiting the bilingual advantage. Developmental Science, 10 , 719-726.
    • Mueller Gathercole, V. C., Thomas, E. M., Kennedy, I., Prys, C., Young, N., Viñas Guasch, N., . . . Jones, L. (2014). Does language dominance affect cognitive performance in bilinguals? Lifespan evidence from preschoolers through older adults on card sorting, Simon and metalinguistic tasks. Frontiers in Psychology, 5 , 1-14.
    • Namazi, M., & Thordardottir, E. (2010). A working memory, not bilingual advantage, in controlled attention. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 13 , 597-616.
    • Office for National Statistics. (2010). Standard occupational classification 2010. volume 3: The national statistics socio-economic classification. Basingstoke: Palgrave MacMillan.
    • Paap, K. R. (2014). The role of componential analysis, categorical hypothesising, replicability and confirmation bias in testing for bilingual advantages in executive functioning. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 26 , 242-255.
    • Paap, K. R., & Greenberg, Z. I. (2013). There is no coherent evidence for a bilingual advantage in executive processing. Cognitive Psychology, 66 , 232-258.
    • Paap, K. R., Johnson, H. A., & Sawi, O. (2014). Are bilingual advantages dependent upon specific tasks or specific bilingual experiences? Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 26 , 615-639.
    • Paap, K. R., & Sawi, O. (2014). Bilingual advantages in executive functioning: Problems in convergent validity, discriminant validity, and the identification of the theoretical constructs. Frontiers in psychology, 5 , 00962.
    • Poarch, G. J., & van Hell, J. G. (2012). Executive functions and inhibitory control in multilingual children: Evidence from second-language learners, bilinguals, and trilinguals. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 113 , 535-551.
    • Prior, A., & Gollan, T. H. (2011). Good language-switchers are good task-switchers: Evidence from Spanish-English and Mandarin-English bilinguals. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 17 , 682-691.
    • Ripatti, S., & Palmgren, J. (2002). Estimation of multivariate frailty models using penalized partial likelihood. Biometics, 56 , 1016-1022.
    • Rosselli, M., Ardila, A., Lalwani, L. N., & Vélez-Uribe, I. (2015). The effect of language proficiency on executive functions in balanced and unbalanced Spanish-English bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, FirstView, 1-15.
    • Scaltritti, M., Peressotti, F., & Miozzo, M. (2015). Bilingual advantage and language switch: What's the linkage? Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, FirstView, 1-18.
    • Simon, J. (1969). Reactions towards the source of stimulation. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 81 , 174-176.
    • Soveri, A., Rodriguez-Fornells, A., & Laine, M. (2011). Is there a relationship between language switching and executive functions in bilingualism? Introducing a within-group analysis approach. Frontiers in Psychology, 2 , 183.
    • Therneau, T. M., Grambsch, P. M., & Pankratz, V. S. (2003). Penalized survival models and frailty. Journal of computational and graphical statistics, 12 , 156-175.
    • Titone, D., Pivneva, I., Sheikh, N. A., Webb, N., & Whitford, V. M. (2015). Doubling down on multifactorial approaches to the study of bilingualism and executive control. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18 , 43-44.
    • Valian, V. (2015). Bilingualism and cognition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18 , 3-24.
    • Verreyt, N., Woumans, E. V. Y., Vandelanotte, D., Szmalec, A., & Duyck, W. (2015). The influence of language-switching experience on the bilingual executive control advantage. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, FirstView, 1-10.
    • Weber, R. C., Johnson, A., Riccio, C. A., & Liew, J. (2015). Balanced bilingualism and executive functioning in children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, FirstView, 1-7.
    • Wechsler, D. (1991). WISC-III: Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children: Manual. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation, Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.
    • Woodard, E., & Rodman, A. (2007). Socio-economic status as a mediating factor in the relationship between bilingualism and the cognitive development of pre-school children. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the National Communication Association 93rd Annual Convention.
    • Yang, S., Yang, H., & Lust, B. (2011). Early childhood bilingualism leads to advances in executive attention: Dissociating culture and language. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 14 , 412-422.
    • Zelazo, P. (2006). The Dimensional Change Card Sort (DCCS): A method of assessing executive function in children. Nature Protocols, 1 , 297-301.
  • No related research data.
  • No similar publications.

Share - Bookmark

Cite this article