OpenAIRE is about to release its new face with lots of new content and services.
During September, you may notice downtime in services, while some functionalities (e.g. user registration, login, validation, claiming) will be temporarily disabled.
We apologize for the inconvenience, please stay tuned!
For further information please contact helpdesk[at]openaire.eu

fbtwitterlinkedinvimeoflicker grey 14rssslideshare1
Heussen, D.; Voorspoels, W.; Verheyen, S.; Storms, G.; Hampton, J. A. (2011)
Publisher: Springer
Languages: English
Types: Article
Subjects: BF, Induction, Negative evidence, Categories, Models of induction
Both intuitively, and according to similarity-based theories of induction, relevant evidence raises argument strength when it is positive and lowers it when it is negative. In three experiments, we tested the hypothesis that argument strength can actually increase when negative evidence is introduced. Two kinds of argument were compared through forced choice or sequential evaluation: single positive arguments (e.g., “Shostakovich’s music causes alpha waves in the brain; therefore, Bach’s music causes alpha waves in the brain”) and double mixed arguments (e.g., “Shostakovich’s music causes alpha waves in the brain, X’s music DOES NOT; therefore, Bach’s music causes alpha waves in the brain”). Negative evidence in the second premise lowered credence when it applied to an item X from the same subcategory (e.g., Haydn) and raised it when it applied to a different subcategory (e.g., AC/DC). The results constitute a new constraint on models of induction.
  • The results below are discovered through our pilot algorithms. Let us know how we are doing!

    • Blok, S. V., Medin, D. L., & Osherson, D. (2007). Induction as conditional probability judgment. Memory & Cognition, 35, 1353-1364.
    • Feeney, A. (2007). How many processes underlie category-based induction? Effects of conclusion specificity and cognitive ability. Memory & Cognition, 35, 1830-1839.
    • Grice, H. P. (1975/1989). Logic and conversation. In H. P. Grice (Ed.), Studies in the way of words (pp. 22-40). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
    • Hampton, J.A. & Cannon, I. (2004). Category-based induction: An effect of conclusion typicality. Memory & Cognition, 32, 235-243.
    • Hayes, B., Heit, E., & Swendsen, H. (2010). Inductive reasoning. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 1, 278-292.
    • Heit, E. (1998). A Bayesian analysis of some forms of inductive reasoning. In M. Oaksford & N. Chater (Eds.), Rational models of cognition (pp. 248-274). Oxford University Press.
    • Heit, E. (2000). Properties of inductive reasoning. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 7, 569-592.
    • Heit, E. Hahn, U. & Feeney, A. (2004) Defending diversity. In W. Ahn, R. L. Goldstone, B. C. Love, A. B. Markman, & P. Wolff, (Eds.), Categorization inside and outside of the lab: Festschrift in honor of Douglas L. Medin. Washington, DC: APA Press.
    • Hempel, C. G. (1945) Studies in the Logic of Confirmation I. Mind, 54, 1-96.
    • Heussen, D. & Hampton, J. A. (2011). Induction with mixed evidence: The role of typicality. Under review.
    • Heussen, D., Voorspoels, W., & Storms, G. (2010). Can similarity-based models of induction handle negative evidence. In S. Ohlsson & R. Catrambone (Eds.), Proceedings of the 32nd Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 2033-2038). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.
    • Houtz, J. C., Moore, J. W., & Davis, J. K. (1973). Effects of different types of positive and negative instances in learning “nondimensioned” concepts. Journal of Educational Psychology, 64, 206-211.
    • Kalish, C. W. & Lawson, C. A. (2007). Negative evidence and inductive generalization. Thinking & Reasoning, 13, 394-425.
    • Kemp, C., & Tenenbaum, J. B. (2009). Structured statistical models of inductive reasoning. Psychological Review, 116, 20-58.
    • McDonald, J., Samuels, M., & Rispoli, J. (1996). A hypothesis assessment model of categorical argument strength. Cognition, 59, 199-217.
    • Medin, D. L., Coley, J. D., Storms, G., & Hayes, B. K. (2003). A relevance theory of induction. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10, 517-532.
    • Osherson, D. N., Smith, E. E., Wilkie, O., Lopez, A., & Shafir, E. (1990). Category-based induction. Psychological Review, 97, 185-200.
    • Rips, L.J. (1975). Inductive judgments about natural categories. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 14, 665-681.
    • Shepard, R. N. (1987). Toward a universal law of generalization for psychological science. Science, 237, 1317-1323.
    • Sloman, S.A. (1993). Feature-based induction. Cognitive Psychology, 25, 231-280.
    • Smith, E.E., Shafir, E., & Osherson, D.N. 1993. Similarity, plausibility, and judgments of probability. Cognition, 49, 67-96.
    • Sperber, D. & Wilson, D. (1995). Relevance: Communication and cognition (2nd ed.) Oxford: Blackwell.
    • Tenenbaum, J. B. & Griffiths, T. L. (2001). Generalization, similarity, and Bayesian inference. Behavioral and brain sciences, 24, 629.
    • Tenenbaum, J. B., Kemp, C. & Shafto, P. (2007). Theory-based Bayesian models of inductive reasoning. In Feeney, A. & Heit, E. (Eds.), Induction. Cambridge, U.K.: Cambridge University Press.
    • Waxman, S.R., Lynch, E.B., Casey, K.L., & Baer, L. (1997). Setters and samoyeds: The emergence of subordinate level categories as a basis for inductive inference. Developmental Psychology, 33, 1074-1090.
  • No related research data.
  • No similar publications.

Share - Bookmark

Cite this article

Cookies make it easier for us to provide you with our services. With the usage of our services you permit us to use cookies.
More information Ok