LOGIN TO YOUR ACCOUNT

Username
Password
Remember Me
Or use your Academic/Social account:

CREATE AN ACCOUNT

Or use your Academic/Social account:

Congratulations!

You have just completed your registration at OpenAire.

Before you can login to the site, you will need to activate your account. An e-mail will be sent to you with the proper instructions.

Important!

Please note that this site is currently undergoing Beta testing.
Any new content you create is not guaranteed to be present to the final version of the site upon release.

Thank you for your patience,
OpenAire Dev Team.

Close This Message

CREATE AN ACCOUNT

Name:
Username:
Password:
Verify Password:
E-mail:
Verify E-mail:
*All Fields Are Required.
Please Verify You Are Human:
fbtwitterlinkedinvimeoflicker grey 14rssslideshare1
Publisher: Wiley
Languages: English
Types: Article
Subjects:
Identifiers:doi:10.1002/ejsp.752
Group biases based on broad category membership appear early in human development. However, like many other primates humans inhabit social worlds also characterised by small groups of social coalitions which are not demarcated by visible signs or social markers. A critical cognitive challenge for a young child is thus how to extract information concerning coalition structure when coalitions are dynamic and may lack stable and outwardly visible cues to membership. Therefore, the ability to decode behavioural cues of affiliations present in everyday social interactions between individuals would have conferred powerful selective advantages during our evolution. This would suggest that such an ability may emerge early in life, however, little research has investigated the developmental origins of such processing. The present paper will review recent empirical research which indicates that in the first 2 years of life infants achieve a host of social-cognitive abilities that make them well adapted to processing coalition-affiliations of others. We suggest that such an approach can be applied to better understand the origins of intergroup attitudes and biases. Copyright © 2010 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.\ud \ud
  • The results below are discovered through our pilot algorithms. Let us know how we are doing!

    • Csibra, G., Bíró, S., Koós, S., & Gergely, G. (2003). One-year-old Infants Use Teleological Representations of Actions Productively. Cognitive Science, 27, 111- 133.
    • Csibra, G., & Gergely, G. (2009), Natural Pedagogy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13 (4), 148-153.
    • Cunningham, W. A., Johnson, M. K., Raye, C. L., Gatenby, J. C., Gore, J. C., & Banaji, M. R. (2004). Separable Neural Components in the Processing of Black and White Faces. Psychological Science, 15 (12), 806-813.
    • de Haan, M., & Nelson, C. A. (1999). Brain Activity Differentiates Face and Object Processing in 6-Month-Old Infants. Developmental Psychology, 35, 1113-1121.
    • de Rosnay, M., Cooper, P. J., Tsigaras, N., & Murray, L. (2006). Transmission of Social Anxiety from Mother to Infant: An experimental study using a social referencing paradigm. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44, 1165-1175.
    • Dunbar, R. I. M. (1993). Coevolution of Neocortical Size, Group Size and Language in Humans. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4): 681-735.
    • Dunham, Y., Baron, A. S., & Banaji, M. R. (2008). The Development of Implicit Intergroup Cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12 (7), 248-253.
    • Dunham, Y. & Banaji, M. R. (2007, February). Poster presented at the 8th Annual Meeting of The Society for Social and Personality Psychology, Memphis, TN.
    • Fagan, J. F. (1973). Infants' Delayed Recognition Memory and Forgetting. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 16, 424-450.
    • Hoehl, S., Wiese, L., & Striano, T. (2008), Young Infants' Neural Processing of Objects Is Affected by Eye Gaze Direction and Emotional Expression. PLoS ONE, 3 (6), 2389.
    • Hornik, R., & Gunnar, M. R. (1987). A Descriptive Analysis of Infant Social Referencing. Child Development, 59 (3), 626-634.
    • Hornik, R., Risenhoover, N., & Gunnar, M., (1987). The Effects of Maternal Positive, Neutral, and Negative Affective Communications on Infant Responses to New Toys. Child Development, 58 (4), 937-944.
    • Johnson, S.C., Shimizu, Y.A., & Ok, S-J. (2007). Actors and Actions: The Role of Agent Behavior in Infants' Attribution of Goals. Cognitive Development 22, 310-322.
    • Kaltenbach, K., Weinraub, M., & Fullard, W. (1980). Infant Wariness Toward Strangers Reconsidered: Infants' and Mothers' Reactions to Unfamiliar Persons. Child Development, 51, 1197-1202.
    • Keating,C., & Bai, D. (1986). Children's Attributions of Social Dominance from Facial Cues .Child Development, 57, 1269-1276.
    • Kelly, D. J., Quinn, P. C., Slater, A. M., Lee, K., Ge, L., & Pascalis, O. (2007). The Other-Race Effect Develops During Infancy. Psychological Science, 18 (12), 1084-1089.
    • Kelly, D. J., Quinn, P. C., Slater, A. M., Lee, K., Gibson, A., Smith, M., Ge, L., & Pascalis, O. (2005). Three-Month-Olds, but not Newborns, Prefer Own-Race Faces. Developmental Science, 8 (6), F31-F36.
    • Kinzler, K. D., Dupoux, E., & Spelke, E. S. (2007). The Native Language of Social Cognition. The Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104, 12577-12580.
    • 6-Month-Old Infants. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 24 (1), 249-260.
    • Premack, D., & Premack, A. J. (1997). Infants Attribute Value± to the Goal-Directed Actions of Self-Propelled Objects. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9, 6, 848- 856.
    • Repacholi, B. M., & Gopnik, A. (1997). Early Reasoning About Desires: Evidence From 14- and 18-Month-Olds. Developmental Psychology, 33 (1), 12-21.
    • Rochat, P., Striano, T., & Morgan, R. (2004). Who is Doing What to Whom? Young Infants' Developing Sense of Social Causality in Animated Displays. Perception, 33, 355-369.
    • Ross, H., & Conant, C. (1992). The Social Structure of Early Conflict: Interaction, Relationships, and Alliances. In C. Shantz & W. Hartup (Eds.), Conflict In Child And Adolescent Development (pp. 153-185). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
    • Ross, H., Conant, C., Cheyne, J., & Alevizos, E. (1992). Relationships and Alliances in the Social Interaction of Kibbutz Toddlers. Social Development, 1, 1-17.
    • Russon, A. E., &Waite, B. E. (1991). Patterns of Dominance and Imitation in an Infant Peer Group. Ethology and Sociobiology, 13, 55-73.
    • Sangrioli, S., & de Schonen, S. (2004). Recognition of Own-Race and Other-Race Faces by Three-Month-Old Infants. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45 (7), 1219-1227.
    • Schlottmann, A., Surian, L., & Ray, E. D. (2009). Causal Perception of Action-andReaction Sequences in 8- to 10-Month-Olds. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 103, 87-107.
  • No related research data.
  • No similar publications.

Share - Bookmark

Cite this article