LOGIN TO YOUR ACCOUNT

Username
Password
Remember Me
Or use your Academic/Social account:

CREATE AN ACCOUNT

Or use your Academic/Social account:

Congratulations!

You have just completed your registration at OpenAire.

Before you can login to the site, you will need to activate your account. An e-mail will be sent to you with the proper instructions.

Important!

Please note that this site is currently undergoing Beta testing.
Any new content you create is not guaranteed to be present to the final version of the site upon release.

Thank you for your patience,
OpenAire Dev Team.

Close This Message

CREATE AN ACCOUNT

Name:
Username:
Password:
Verify Password:
E-mail:
Verify E-mail:
*All Fields Are Required.
Please Verify You Are Human:
fbtwitterlinkedinvimeoflicker grey 14rssslideshare1
Languages: English
Types: Doctoral thesis
Subjects: BF0309, P0101
Pause analysis is a method that investigates processes of writing by measuring the amount of time between pen strokes. It provides the field of second language studies with a means to explore the cognitive processes underpinning the nature of writing. This study examined the potential of using free handwritten copying of sentences as a means of investigating components of the cognitive processes of adults who have English as their Second Language (ESL).\ud \ud A series of one pilot and three experiments investigated possible measures of language skill and the factors that influence the quality of the measures. The pilot study, with five participants of varying English competence, identified copying without pre-reading to be an effective task and ‘median’ at the beginning of words to be an effective measure. Experiment 1 (n=20 Malaysian speakers) found jumbled sentences at the letter and word levels to effectively differentiate test-taker competence in relation to grammatical knowledge. Experiment 2 (n=20 Spanish speakers) investigated the jumbling effects further, but found that participants varied their strategy depending on the order of the sentence types. As a result, Experiment 3 (n= 24 Malaysian speakers) used specific task instructions to control participant strategy use, so that they either attended to the meaning of the sentences, or merely copied as quickly as possible. Overall, these experiments show that it is feasible to apply pause analysis to cognitively investigate both grammar and vocabulary components of language processing.\ud \ud Further, a theoretical information processing model of copying (MoC) was developed. The model assists in the analysis and description of (1) the flow of copying processes; (2) the factors that might affect longer or shorter pauses amongst participants of varying competence level; and (3) sentence stimuli design.
  • The results below are discovered through our pilot algorithms. Let us know how we are doing!

    • Ahlsen, E. and Stromqvist, S. (1999). ScriptLog: A tool for logging the writing process and its possible diagnostic use. Paper presented at the 1998 ISAAC Research Symposium, Dublin, Ireland
    • Akamatsu, N. (1999). The effects of first language orthographic features on word recognition processing in English as a second language. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 11(4), 381-403.
    • Alamargot, D., & L. Chanquoy. (2001). Through the models of writing. Dordrecht: Kluwer.
    • Alamargot, D., Chesnet, D., Dansac, C., & Ros, C. (2006). Eye and pen: A new device for studying reading during writing. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 38, 287-299.
    • Alderson, J.C. & Banerjee, J. (2001).Language testing and assessment (Part 1).Language Teaching, [Online].34(213-236), Available at: http://eprints.lancs.ac.uk/1036/1/displayFulltext2.pdf [Accessed August 2009]
    • Alderson, J.C. & Banerjee, J. (2002).Language testing and assessment (Part 2).Language Teaching, [Online].3(213-236), Available at: http://journals.cambridge.org (Cambridge Journals Online) [Accessed August 2009]
    • Alderson, J.C. (2000). Assessing reading ability. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
    • Alexander, P.A., & Jetton, T.L. (2000). Learning from text: A multi-dimensional and developmental perspective. In M. L. Kamil, P. B. Mosenthal, P. D. Pearson, & R. Barr (Eds.), Handbook of reading research: Vol. III (pp. 285-3 10). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
    • Álvarez, C.J., Carreiras, M., de Vega, M. (2000). Syllable-frequency effect in visual word recognition: Evidence of sequential-type processing. Psicológica 21. 3: 341-374.
    • Anderson, N. J. (1991). Individual differences in strategy use in second language reading and testing. The modern language journal, 75(4), 460-472.
    • Anderson, R.C. and Freebody, P. (1979). Vocabulary knowledge. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED 177 480).
    • Andersson, B., Dahl, J., Holmqvist, K., Holsanova, J., Johansson, V., Karlsson, H. (2006). Combining keystroke logging with eye tracking. In L. Van Waes, M. Leiten, & C. M. Neuwirth (Eds.), Writing and digital media (pp. 166-172). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
    • Askov, E.N. (1975) Handwriting: copying versus tracing as the most effective type of practice. Journal of Baddeley, A.D. & Hitch, G. (1974). Working memory. In G.H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory (vol.8, pp. 281-301). London: Academic Press.
    • Baddeley, A.D., Thomson, N., & Buchanan, M. (1975).Word length and the structure of short term memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behaviour, 14, 575-589
    • Baddeley, A. D., & Logie, R. H. (1999). Working memory: The multiple-component model. In A. Miyake & P. Shah (Eds.), Models of working memory (pp. 28±61). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
    • Barbe, W.B., Lucas, V.H. & Wasylyk, Th.M. (1984). Handwriting: Basic skills for effective communication, Zaner-Bloser, Columbus, OH
    • Birch, B. (2007). English L2 reading: Getting to the bottom (2nd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers.
    • Bonin, P. (1997). Produire des mots isolés oralement et par écrit. Revue de Neuropsychologie, 7, 29-70.
    • Bonin, P., Fayol, M., & Gombert, J.E. (1997). Role of phonological and orthographic codes in picture naming and writing: An interference paradigm study. Current Psychology of Cognition, 16, 299-320.
    • Bourdin, B., & Fayol, M. (1994). Is written language production more difficult than oral language production? A working memory approach. International Journal of Psychology, 29, 591-620.
    • Bridwell, L. S., Sirc, G., & Brooke, R. (1985). Revising and composing: Case studies of student writers. In S. Freedman (Ed.), The acquisition of written language: Revision and response (pp. 172-194). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
    • Bridwell-Bowles, L. S., Johnson, P. and Brehe, S. (1987). Computers and composing: Case studies of experienced writers. In A. Matsuhashi (ed.) Writing in Real Time: Modelling Production Processes (pp. 81-107). New York: Longman.
    • Brinkmann, E. (2004). Abschreiben üben. [Practising copying.]. Grundschule Deutsch, 2, 29.
    • Butterworth, B. (1980). Some constraints on models of language production. In B. Butterworth (Ed.), Language production: Vol. 1. Speech and talk. New York: Academic Press.
    • Carroll, J.B. (1965). The prediction of success in intensive foreign language training. In R.Glaser (Ed.), Training, research, and education (pp. 87-136). New York: Wiley.
    • Chanquoy, L., Foulin, J.-N., & Fayol, M. (1990). Temporal management of short text writing by children and adults. Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive, 10, 513-540.
    • Cheng, P. C. H., & Rojas-Anaya, H. (2005). Writing out a temporal signal of chunks: patterns of pauses reflect the induced structure of written number sequences. In B. G. Bara, L. Barsalou & M. Bucciarelli (Eds.), Proceedings of the Twenty Seventh Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 424-429). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
    • Cheng, P. C. H., & Rojas-Anaya, H. (2006). A temporal signal reveals chunk structure in the writing of word phrases. In Proceedings of the Twenty Eighth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
    • Cheng, P. C. H., & Rojas-Anaya, H. (2007).Measuring Mathematical Formula Writing Competence: An Application of Graphical Protocol Analysis. In D. S. McNamara & J. G. Trafton (Eds.), Proceedings of the Twenty Ninth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 869-874). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.
    • Cheng, P. C. H., & Rojas-Anaya, H. (2008). A Graphical Chunk Production Model: Evaluation Using Graphical Protocol Analysis with Artificial Sentences. In B. C. Love, K. McRae & V. M. Sloutsky (Eds.), Proceedings of the Thirtieth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 1972-1977). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.
    • Cheng, P. C.-H., McFadzean, J., & Copeland, L. (2001).Drawing out the temporal structure of induced perceptual chunks. In J. D. Moore & K. Stenning (Eds.), Proceedings of the Twenty Third Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 200-205). Mahwah, New Jersey: Lawrence Erbaum.
    • Cheng, P.C-H., & Rojas-Anaya, H. (2004). TRACE user guide (Unpublished Representational Systems Laboratory report).
    • Christianson, K., Johnson, R. L., & Rayner, K. (2005). Letter transposition within and across morphemes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31, 1327-1339
    • Cohen, A. D., & Upton, T. A. (2006). Strategies in responding to new TOEFL reading tasks. TOEFL Monograph Series (MS-33). Princeton, NJ: ETS.
    • Coltheart, M. (1978). Lexical access in simple reading tasks. In G. Underwood (Ed.), Strategies of information processing (pp. 151-216). New York: Academic Press.
    • Coltheart, M., Curtis, B., Atkins, P., & Haller, M. (1993). Models of reading aloud: Dual-route and parallel distributed-processing approaches. Psychological Review, 100, 589-608.
    • Coltheart, M., Rastle, K., Perry, C., Langdon, R., & Ziegler, J. (2001). DRC: A dual route cascaded model of Cook, V. (1992). Evidence for multicompetence. Language Learning, 42(4):557-91.
    • Ellis, A.W., Young, A.W. (1988). Human cognitive neuropsychology. Erlbaum, London (1988)
    • Fang, S.-P., Horng, R.-Y., & Tzeng, O. J. L. (1986). Consistency effects in the Chinese character and pseudocharacter naming task. In H. S. R. Kao & R. Hoosain (Eds.), Linguistics, psychology, and the Chinese character (pp. 11-21). Hong Kong: University of Hong Kong, Center of Asian Studies.
    • Fayol, M. (1998). From on-line management problems to strategies in written composition. In G. Rijlaarsdam & E. Esperet (Series Ed.) & G. Jefferey (Vol. Eds.) Studies in Writing: Vol. 3. The Cognitive Demands of Writing. (pp. 13-23). Amsterdam: Amsterdam University Press.
    • Foulin, J.-N. (1998). To what extent does pause location predict pause duration in adults' and children's writing? Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive, 17, 601-620.
    • Glushko, R. J. (1979). The organization and activation of orthographic knowledge in reading aloud. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 5, 674-691.
    • Gonzalez, C., Anderson, J., Culmer, P., Burke, M.R., & Mon-Williams, M. (2011). Is tracing or copying better when learning to reproduce a pattern? Experimental Brain Research, Vol. 208 (3) 459-465.
    • Goodman, K.S. (1967). Reading: a psycholinguistic guessing game. Journal of the Reading Specialist, 4, 126-35.
    • Gough, P. B. (1972). One second of reading. In J. F. Kavanagh & G. Mattingly (Eds.), Language by ear and by eye (pp. 331-358). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
    • Grabe, W. & Stoller, F.L. (2002). Teaching and researching reading London: Longman.
    • Grabowski, J. (2008). The internal structure of university students' keyboard skills. Journal of Writing Research, 1, 27-52.
    • Grabowski, J. (2009). Speaking, writing, and memory span in children: Output modality affects cognitive performance. International Journal of Psychology, in press.
    • Grabowski, J., Blabusch, C. & Lorenz, Th. (2007). Welche Schreibkompetenz? - Handschrift und Tastatur in der Hauptschule. [Which writing competence? - Handwriting and keyboard in German Hauptschule.]. In M.Becker-Mrotzek & K.Schindler (Eds.), Texte schreiben. (pp. 41-61). Kölner Beiträge zur Sprachdidaktik, 5. Köln: Gilles & Francke.
    • Grabowski, J., Weinzierl, C., & Schmitt, M. (2010). Second and fourth graders' copying ability: From graphical to linguistic processing. Journal of Research in Reading, 33, 39-53.
    • Graham, S., & Harris, K. R. (2000). The role of self-regulation and transcription skills in writing and writing development. Educational Psychologist, 35(1), 3-12.
    • Graham, S., Berninger, V. W., Abbot, R. D., Abbot, S. P., & Whitaker, D. (1997). Role of mechanics in composing of elementary school children: A new methodological approach. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 170-182.
    • Grainger, J. & Whitney, C. (2004) Does the huamn mnid raed wrods as a wlohe? Trends Cognition. Sci. 8, 58-59
    • Hayes, J.R. & Flower, L. (1980). 'Identifying the organisation of the writing process'. In L. W. Gregg & E. R Steinberg (eds.). Cognitive processes in writing, 3-30. Hillsdale, NJ: Larence Erlbaum Associates.
    • Haynes, M., & Carr, T. H. (1990). Writing system background and second language reading: A component skills analysis of English reading by native speaker-readers of Chinese. In T. H. Carr (Ed.), Reading and its development: Component skills approaches (pp. 375-421). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
    • Herrick, V.E. and Okada, N. (1963). The present scene: Practices in the teaching of handwriting in the United States. In V.E. Herrick (Ed.), New horizons for research in handwriting, University of Wisconsin Press, Madison, WI
    • Hino, Y., & Lupker, S. J. (1998). The effects of word frequency for Japanese kana and kanji words in naming and lexical decisions: Can the dual-route model save the lexical selection account. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 1431-1453.
    • Hollerbach, J.M. (1981). An oscillation theory of handwriting. Biological Cybernetics, 39, pp. 139-156
    • Holm, A., & Dodd, B. (1996). The effect of first written language on the acquisition of English literacy. Cognition, 59(2), 119-147.
    • Hudson, R.F., Pullen, P.C., Lane, H.B., & Torgesen, J.K. (2009). The complex nature of reading fluency: A multidimensional view. Reading & Writing Quarterly, 25(1), 4-32
    • Hue, C. W. (1992). Recognition processing in character naming. In H. C. Chen & O. J. L. Tzeng (Eds.), Language processing in Chinese (pp. 93-107). Amsterdam: North-Holland.
    • Jackson, N. E., Lu, W. H., & Ju, D. (1994). Reading Chinese and reading English: Similarities, differences, and second-language reading. In V. W. Berninger (Ed.), The varieties of orthographic knowledge 1: Theoretical and developmental issues. Neuropsychology and cognition (Vol. 8, pp. 73-109). Norwell, MA: Kluwer Academic.
    • Kandel, S., Herault, L., Grosjacques, G., Lambert, E., Fayol, M. (2009). Orthographic vs. phonologic syllables in handwriting production. Cognition 110. 3: 440-444.
    • Kandel, S., Álvarez, C.J., Vallée, N. (2006). Syllables as processing units in handwriting production. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 32. 1: 18-31.
    • Kao, H. S. R., Shek, D. T. L., & Lee, E. S. P. (1983). Control modes and task complexity in tracing and handwriting performance. Acta Psychologica, 54, 69e77.
    • Kao, H.S.R., Mak, P.H. & Lam P.W. (1986). Handwriting pressure: Effects of task complexity, control mode and orthographic difference. In H.S.R. Kao, G.P. van Galen, R. Hoosain (Eds.), Graphonomics: Contemporary research in handwriting, North-Holland, Amsterdam
    • Kao, H.S.R., van Galen, G.P. & Hoosain, R. (1986). Graphonomics. Contemporary research in handwriting, North-Holland, Amsterdam.
    • Kellogg, R.T. (1998). Components of Working Memory in Text Production. In G. Rijlaarsdam& E. Esperet (Series Ed.) & G. Jefferey (Vol. Eds.) Studies in Writing: Vol. 3. The Cognitive Demands of Writing. (pp. 43-62). Amsterdam: Amsterdam University Press.
    • Khalidieh, S.A. (2001). The Relationship between Knowledge of Icraab, Lexical Knowledge, and Reading Comprehension of Non-native Readers of Arabic. The Modern Language Journal, Volume 85 (3), 416-431.
    • Kintsch, W., & van Dijk, T. A. (1978). Toward a model of text comprehension and production.
    • Lambert, E., Kandel, S., Fayol, M., & Esperet, E. (2008). The effect of the number of syllables when writing poly-syllabic words. Reading & Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 21, 859-883.
    • Laufer, B. & Goldstein, Z. (2004). Testing Vocabulary Knowledge: Size, Strength, and Computer Adaptiveness. Language Learning 54: 469-523
    • Laufer, B., & Nation, P. (1995). Vocabulary size & use: Lexical richness in L2 written productions. Applied Linguistics 16 (3), 307-322.
    • Laufer, B., Elder, C., Hill, K. & Congdon, P. (2004). Size and strength: Do we need both to measure vocabulary knowledge? Language Testing Testing 21: 202-226
    • Lewis, R. (1993). An Architecturally-based Theory of Human Sentence Processing. PhD Thesis, Carnegie Lindgren, E., & Sullivan, K. P. H. (2006). Analysing on-line revision. In K. P. H. Sullivan & E. Lindgren (Eds.), Computer key-stroke logging: Methods and applications (pp. 157-188). Oxford: Elsevier.
    • Longcamp, M., Anton, J.L. & Velay, J.L. (2003). Visual presentation of single letters activates a pre-motor area involved in writing. Neuroimage, 19, 1492-1500.
    • Longcamp, M., Boucard, C., Gilhodes, J.C. & Velay, J.C. (2006) Remembering the orientation of newly learned characters depends on the associated writing knowledge: a comparison between handwriting and typing. Hum Mov Sci 25:646-656
    • Longcamp, M., Boucard, C., Gilhodes, J.C., Anton, J.L., Roth, M., Nazarian, B. & Velay JL (2008). Learning through hand- or typewriting influences visual recognition of new graphic shapes: behavioral and functional imaging evidence. J CognNeurosci20:802-815
    • Longcamp, M., Zerbato-Poudou, M.T. & Velay, J.L. (2005). The influence of writing practice on letter recognition in preschool children: a comparison between handwriting and typing. ActaPsychol119:67-79
    • Lorenz, T. & Grabowski, J. (2009). Handschrift oder Tastatur in der Hauptschule. Ökonomie und Mythen von Schriftmedien. [Handwriting or keyboard in German Hauptschule. Economy and myths of writing media.]. In K.Schneider, G.Schwab & M.Weingardt (Eds.), Hauptschulforschung konkret. (pp. 147-157). Baltmannsweiler: Schneider Verlag Hohengehren.
    • M. Peck, E.N. Askov & S.H. Fairchild. (1980). Another decade of research in handwriting: Progress and Prospect in the 1970s. Journal of Educational Research, 73 (1980), pp. 283-298
    • Marshall, J.C. & Newcombe, F. (1973). Patterns of paralexia: A psycholinguistic approach. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 2, 175-199.
    • Matsuhashi, A. (1981). Pausing and planning: The tempo of written discourse production. Research in the Teaching of English, 15(2), 113-134.
    • Matsuhashi, A. (1982). Explorations in the real-time production of written discourse. In: M. Nystrand (Ed.), What writers know. The language, process, and structure of written discourse (pp. 269-290). New York: Academic Press, Inc.
    • Matsuhashi, A. (1987). Revising the plan and altering the text. In: A. Matsuhashi (Ed.), Writing in real time - modelling production processes (pp. 197-223). Norwood, NJ: Ablex Publishing company.
    • McCutchen, D. (1996). A capacity theory of writing: Working memory in composition. Educational McCutchen, D. (2000). Knowledge, processing, and working memory: Implications for a theory of writing. Educational Psychologist, 35(1), 13-23.
    • Meara, P. & Fitzpatrick, T. (2000). Lex30: An improved method of assessing productive vocabulary in an L2. System 28,1, 19-30.
    • Meara, P. (1992). EFL vocabulary tests. Unpublished manuscript, Centre for Applied Language Studies, University College, Swansea, UK.
    • Medwell, J. & Wray, D. (2007). Handwriting: What do know and what do we need to know? Literacy, Vol. 41, No. 1. Handwriting (pp. 10-15). Blackwell Publishing.
    • Mehler, J. (1963). Some effects of grammatical transformations on the recall of English sentences. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 4,748-762.
    • Nation, I.S.P. (2007). The four strands. Innovation in Language Learning and Teaching 1, 1: 1-12.
    • Paris, S.G., Lawton, T.A., Turner, J.C., & Roth, J.L. (1991). A developmental perspective on standardised achievement testing. Educational Researcher, 20(5), 12-20.
    • Patterson, K. E. & Shewell, C. (1987). “Speak and spell: Dissociations and word class effects”. In The cognitive neuropsychology of language, Edited by: Coltheart, M., Job, R. and Sartori, G. Hove, , UK: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Ltd.
    • Pellicer-Sanchez, A. (2011). "Using eye-tracking methodology to measure L2 vocabulary acquisition from reading." Innovative approaches to language testing research [Conference]. BAAL TEASig 2011.
    • Pennington, M.C. (1999). 'Phonology in the context of communication and language learning', Research Report, Series 2. University of Luton.
    • Perfetti, C. A. (1999). Comprehending written language: A blueprint of the reader. In C. Brown & P. Hagoot (Eds.), The neurocognition of language (pp. 167-208). New York: Oxford University Press.
    • Perfetti, C. A., & Zhang, S. (1995). Very early phonological activation in Chinese reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 24-33.
    • Perfetti, C.A. (1999). Comprehending written language: A blueprint of the reader. In C. Brown & P. Hagoot (Eds.), The neurocognition of language (pp. 167-208). New York: Oxford University Press.
    • Plaut, D.C., McClelland, J.L., Seidenberg, M.S., & Patterson, K. (1996). Understanding normal and impaired word reading: Computational principles in quasi-regular domains.Psychological Review, 103, 56- 115.
    • Power, M. J. (1986). A technique for measuring processing load during speech production. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 15, 371-382.
    • R. Plamondon, C.Y. Suen, M.L. Simner (Eds.). (1989). Computer recognition and human production of handwriting, World Scientific Publishing Co, Singapore
    • R. Sassoon, I. Nimmo-Smith, A.M. Wing. (1986) An analysis of children's penholds. H.S.R. Kao, G.P. van Galen, R. Hoosain (Eds.), Graphonomics: Contemporary research in handwriting, North-Holland, Amsterdam (1986)
    • Rawlinson, G. E. (1976) The significance of letter position in word recognition. Unpublished PhD Thesis, Psychology Department, University of Nottingham, Nottingham UK.
    • Rawlinson, G. E. (1999). " Reibadailty”, New Scientist (vol 162 issue 2188), May 1999, p.55.
    • Rayner, K., & A. Pollatsek. 1989. The psychology of reading. New York: Prentice-Hall.
    • Rayner, K., White, S. J., Johnson, R. L., & Liversedge, S. P. (2006). Raeding wrods with jubmled lettres: There is a cost. Psychological Science, 17, 192-193.
    • Recht, D.R. & Leslie, L. (1988).Effect of prior knowledge on good and poor readers' memory of text. Journal of Educational Psychology, 80:16-20.
    • Rieben, L. & Saada-Robert, M. (1997). Relations between word-search strategies and word-copying strategies in children aged 5 to 6 years old. In C. A.Perfetti, L.Rieben & M.Fayol (Eds.), Learning to spell: Research, theory, and practice across languages. (pp. 295-318). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
    • Rieben, L., Meyer, A. & Perregaux, C. (1991). Individual differences and lexical representations: How five 6-year-old children search for and copy word. L. Rieben, C. Perfetti, Editors , Learning to read-Basic research and its implications, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ (1991), pp. 85-101.
    • Scarborough, H. S. (2001). Connecting early language and literacy to later reading (dis)abilities: Evidence, theory, and practice. In S. Neuman & D. Dickinson (Eds.), Handbook for research in early literacy (pp. 97-110). New York: Guilford Press.
    • Schilperoord, J. (2001). On the cognitive status of pauses in discourse production. In: G. Rijlaarsdam (Series ed.) & T. Olive & C.M. Levy (Vol. eds.), Studies in Writing: Volume 10: Contemporary Tools and Techniques for Studying Writing. Kluwer Academic Publishers: Netherlands.
    • Schilperoord, J. (2003). On the cognitive status of pauses in discourse production. In T. Olive & C. M. Levy (Eds.), Contemporary tools and techniques for studying writing (pp. 61-88). Dordrecht: Kluwer.
    • Schreuder, R., & Baayen, R. H. (1997). How complex simplex words can be. Journal of Memory and Language, 37, 118-139.
    • Sears, C. R., Sharp, C. R., & Lupker, S. J. (2006). Is there a neighbourhood frequency effect in English? Evidence from reading and lexical decision. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 32, 1040-1062.
    • Simpson, S., & Torrance, M. (2007). EyeWrite (Version 5.1). Osgoode, ON: SR Research, and Nottingham, U.K.: Nottingham Trent University.
    • Smith, F. (1971). Understanding reading. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston. 77, 4, 257-273.
    • Spelman Miller, K. & Sullivan, K. P. H. (2006). Keystroke logging: an Introduction. In G. Rijlaarsdam (Series Ed.) and K.P.H. Sullivan, & E. Lindgren. (Vol. Eds.), Studies in Writing, Vol.18, Computer Keystroke Logging: Methods and Applications. Oxford; Elsevier.
    • Süselbeck, G. (2003). Abschreiben: eine wichtige Arbeitstechnik. [Copying: an important working technique.]. Grundschule, 35 (2), 16-20.
    • VanOrden,G.C. & Goldinger,S.D. (1996). Phonologic mediation in skilled and dyslexic reading. In C. Chase, G. Rosen, & G. Sherman (Eds.), Developmental dyslexia: Neuro, cognitive, and genetic mechanisms (pp. 185-223). Timonium, MA: York Press.
    • Velan, H., & Frost, R. (2007). Cambridge University vs. Hebrew University: The impact of letter transposition on reading English and Hebrew. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 913-918.
    • Verhoeven, L., Schreuder, R. & Haarman, V. (2006). Prefix identification in the reading of Dutch bisyllabic words. Reading and Writing, 19 (2006), pp. 651-668
    • Verhoeven, L., Schreuder, R., Baayen, R.H. (2006) Learnability of graphotactic rules in visual word identification. Learning and Instruction, 16 (6), pp. 538-548.
    • Wengelin, A., Torrance, M., Holmqvist, K., Simpson, S., Galbraith, D., Johansson, V., Johansson, R. (2009) Combined eyetracking and keystroke-logging methods for studying cognitive processes in text production. Behavior Research Methods Journal, 41 (2), 337-351.
    • Wharton, G. (2000). Language learning strategy use of bilingual foreign language learners in Singapore. Language learning, 50(2), 203-243.
  • No related research data.
  • Discovered through pilot similarity algorithms. Send us your feedback.

Share - Bookmark

Download from

Cite this article