LOGIN TO YOUR ACCOUNT

Username
Password
Remember Me
Or use your Academic/Social account:

CREATE AN ACCOUNT

Or use your Academic/Social account:

Congratulations!

You have just completed your registration at OpenAire.

Before you can login to the site, you will need to activate your account. An e-mail will be sent to you with the proper instructions.

Important!

Please note that this site is currently undergoing Beta testing.
Any new content you create is not guaranteed to be present to the final version of the site upon release.

Thank you for your patience,
OpenAire Dev Team.

Close This Message

CREATE AN ACCOUNT

Name:
Username:
Password:
Verify Password:
E-mail:
Verify E-mail:
*All Fields Are Required.
Please Verify You Are Human:
fbtwitterlinkedinvimeoflicker grey 14rssslideshare1
Publisher: CEUR Workshop Proceedings
Languages: English
Types: Unknown
Subjects: Mind-wandering, Working memory, Attention, Quality control checking, Short-term memory
Labelling errors on fresh produce are estimated to cost the UK supermarket industry £50m per year in product recalls and wastage. Such errors occur despite robust quality control procedures. Given the financial and environmental impact of these errors, it is important to understand whether label-checking performance can be predicted by individual differences in cognitive abilities. To this end, participants carried out a simulated label-checking task together with a number of measures of information processing speed, attention, short-term/working memory, and mind-wandering. Accuracy of label checking was found to be significantly predicted by three of the measures, with better short-term verbal memory being most strongly associated with performance. Cognitive tests such as these provide a means of identifying how well employees are likely to perform when undertaking such tasks and, if necessary, how they should be supported in that role, possibly forming a screening battery when recruiting new quality control staff. The findings highlight the importance of determining the component processes of cognition which contribute to performance in real-world work environments.
  • The results below are discovered through our pilot algorithms. Let us know how we are doing!

    • Anastasi, A. & Urbina, S. (1997). Psychological Testing (7th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.
    • Baddeley, A. (2003). Working memory: Looking back and looking forward. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4, 829- 839.
    • Bosco, F., Allen, D. G., & Singh, K. (2015). Executive attention: An alternative perspective on general mental ability, performance, and subgroup differences. Personnel Psychology. DOI: 10.1111/peps.12099
    • Corsi, P. (1973). Human memory and the medial temporal region of the brain. Dissertation Abstracts International, 34(2-B), 891.
    • Cowan, N. (2008). What are the differences between longterm, short-term, and working memory? Progress in Brain Research, 169, 323-338.
    • Della Sala, S., Gray, C., Baddeley, A., Allamano, N., & Wilson, L. (1999). Pattern span: A tool for unwelding visuo-spatial memory. Neuropsychologia, 37, 1189-1199.
    • Engle, R. W. (2002). Working memory capacity as executive attention. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11, 19-23.
    • Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Sommer, T., Raz, A., & Posner, M. I. (2002). Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional networks. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, 340-347.
    • Hambrick, D. Z., Oswald, F. L., Darowski, E. S., Rench, T. A., & Brou, R. (2010). Predictors of multitasking performance in a synthetic work paradigm. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 24, 1149-1167.
    • Koshino, H., Carpenter, P. A., Minshew, N. J., Cherkassky, V. L., Keller, T. A., & Just, M. A. (2005). Functional connectivity in an fMRI working memory task in highfunctioning autism. Neuroimage, 24, 810-821.
    • Logie, R. H. (1993). Working memory in everyday cognition. In G. M. Davies and R. H. Logie (Eds.), Memory in everyday life (pp. 367-401). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
    • McKiernan, K. A., D'Angelo, B. R., Kaufman, J. N., Binder, J. R. (2006). Interrupting the stream of consciousness: An fMRI investigation. Neuroimage 29, 1185-1191.
    • Miller, G. A. (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological Review, 63, 81-97.
    • Salthouse, T. A., & Babcock, R. L. (1991). Decomposing adult age differences in working memory. Developmental Psychology, 27, 763-776.
    • Schmidt, F. L. (2002). The role of general cognitive ability and job performance: Why there cannot be a debate. Human Performance, 15, 187-210.
    • Singer, J. L., & Antrobus, J. S. (1970). Imaginal Processes Inventory. Princeton, NJ: Educational Testing Service.
    • Smith-Spark, J. H., Katz, H. B., Marchant, A., & Wilcockson, T. (2015). Label-checking strategies to adapt behaviour to design. Proceedings paper accepted for ECCE 2015: The 33rd annual conference of the European Association of Cognitive Ergonomics, Warsaw, Poland, 1- 3 July.
    • Treisman, A., & Souther, J. (1985). Search asymmetry: A diagnostic for preattentive processing of separable features. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 114, 285-310.
    • Wolfe, J. M. (2001). Asymmetries in visual search: An introduction. Perception & Psychophysics, 63, 381-389.
  • No related research data.
  • No similar publications.

Share - Bookmark

Cite this article