LOGIN TO YOUR ACCOUNT

Username
Password
Remember Me
Or use your Academic/Social account:

CREATE AN ACCOUNT

Or use your Academic/Social account:

Congratulations!

You have just completed your registration at OpenAire.

Before you can login to the site, you will need to activate your account. An e-mail will be sent to you with the proper instructions.

Important!

Please note that this site is currently undergoing Beta testing.
Any new content you create is not guaranteed to be present to the final version of the site upon release.

Thank you for your patience,
OpenAire Dev Team.

Close This Message

CREATE AN ACCOUNT

Name:
Username:
Password:
Verify Password:
E-mail:
Verify E-mail:
*All Fields Are Required.
Please Verify You Are Human:
fbtwitterlinkedinvimeoflicker grey 14rssslideshare1
Liddle, Elizabeth B.
Languages: English
Types: Unknown
Subjects:
It was hypothesized that the deficits underlying reading impairment may arise from supra-modal deficits in temporal and spatial attention, disrupting, on the one hand, the ability to segment the temporally ordered phonemes of language and thus the acquisition of decoding skills, and, on the other, the ability to integrate spatially and temporally ordered orthographic information acquired from the fluent visual scanning of written text. Temporal and spatial attentional deficits in dyslexia were investigated using a lateralized visual temporal order judgment (TOJ) paradigm that allowed both sensitivity to temporal order and spatial attentional bias to be measured. Dyslexic and non-dyslexic participants were required to report the temporal order of two simple visual stimuli presented in either the same or different lateral hemifields. Findings indicated that dyslexic participants showed markedly impaired sensitivity to temporal order, and that the degree of impairment was correlated with the severity of their dyslexia. Furthermore, the findings suggested that at least three partially dissociated deficits may underlie both impaired TOJ task performance and reading disorder. One is a deficit associated with difficulty in reporting the temporal order of two visual stimuli, particularly when the first is presented in right hemifield; with slow word recognition and non-word reading; and with deficits in spelling and phonological skill. This constellation of deficits was interpreted as reflecting deficits in networks in left cerebral hemisphere implicated in phoneme-grapheme mapping and visual orienting. The second is a deficit that is associated with a rightward attentional bias; with inaccurate non-word reading that is worse than predicted by phonological skill or by word recognition; and with poor sustained attention. This constellation of impairments was interpreted as evidence of a deficit in right-lateralised networks implicated in the modulation of arousal, and possibly reflecting a “developmental left-neglect” syndrome. A third deficit was associated with impaired temporal order sensitivity, regardless of hemifield presentation; with symptoms of Attentional Deficit and Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD); and with increased interference from distractor stimuli. This constellation of deficits suggests that the impaired network is implicated in executive control of attention, including conflict resolution and working memory. The results of the investigation as a whole suggests that the reading impairments of dyslexia may arise from attentional deficits that have with substantial overlap with those of ADHD, and include deficits in attentional networks implicated in orienting attention to temporally presented stimuli.
  • The results below are discovered through our pilot algorithms. Let us know how we are doing!

    • American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: American Psychiatric Association, 1994.
    • Amitay S, Ben-Yehudah G, Banai K, Ahissar M. Disabled readers suffer from visual and auditory impairments but not from a specific magnocellular deficit. Brain 2002; 125: 2272-2285.
    • Arduino LS, Burani C, Vallar G. Reading aloud and lexical decision in neglect dyslexia patients: a dissociation. Neuropsychologia 2003; 41: 877-885.
    • Auclair L, Sieroff E. Attentional cueing effect in the identification of words and pseudowords of different length. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section a-Human Experimental Psychology 2002; 55: 445-463.
    • Bakker DJ. Dyslexia and the Ecological Brain. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology 1994; 16: 734-743.
    • Barkley RA. Issues in the diagnosis of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in children. Brain & Development 2003; 25: 77-83.
    • Ben-Yehudah G, Banai K, Ahissar M. Patterns of deficit in auditory temporal processing among dyslexic adults. Neuroreport 2004; 15: 627-631.
    • Ben-Yehudah G, Sackett E, Malchi-Ginzberg L, Ahissar M. Impaired temporal contrast sensitivity in dyslexics is specific to retain-andcompare paradigms. Brain 2001; 124: 1381-1395.
    • Berger A, Posner MI. Pathologies of brain attentional networks. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 2000; 24: 3-5.
    • Biscaldi M, Fischer B, Hartnegg K. Voluntary saccadic control in dyslexia. Perception 2000; 29: 509-521.
    • Biscaldi M, Gezeck S, Stuhr V. Poor saccadic control correlates with dyslexia. Neuropsychologia 1998; 36: 1189-1202.
    • Blakemore SJ, Frith CD, Wolpert DM. The cerebellum is involved in predicting the sensory consequences of action. Neuroreport 2001; 12: 1879-1884.
    • Booth JR, Burman DD, Meyer JR, Gitelman DF, Parrish TB, Mesulam MM. Relation between brain activation and lexical performance. Human Brain Mapping 2003; 19: 155-169.
    • Brannan JR, Williams MC. Developmental Versus Sensory Deficit Effects on Perceptual Processing in the Reading Disabled. Perception & Psychophysics 1988; 44: 437-444.
    • Brown TE. The Brown Attention-Deficit Disorder Scales (Brown ADD Scales) for Adolescents and Adults. San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation: Harcourt Brace, 1996.
  • No related research data.
  • No similar publications.

Share - Bookmark

Cite this article