LOGIN TO YOUR ACCOUNT

Username
Password
Remember Me
Or use your Academic/Social account:

CREATE AN ACCOUNT

Or use your Academic/Social account:

Congratulations!

You have just completed your registration at OpenAire.

Before you can login to the site, you will need to activate your account. An e-mail will be sent to you with the proper instructions.

Important!

Please note that this site is currently undergoing Beta testing.
Any new content you create is not guaranteed to be present to the final version of the site upon release.

Thank you for your patience,
OpenAire Dev Team.

Close This Message

CREATE AN ACCOUNT

Name:
Username:
Password:
Verify Password:
E-mail:
Verify E-mail:
*All Fields Are Required.
Please Verify You Are Human:
fbtwitterlinkedinvimeoflicker grey 14rssslideshare1
Tapper, K.; Pothos, E. M.; Lawrence, A. D. (2010)
Languages: English
Types: Article
Subjects: BF
Identifiers:doi:10.1037/a0020305
Appraisal theories of emotion predict that the relevance of a stimulus to a person's needs and goals influences attentional allocation. We used a modified visual probe task to examine the influence of hunger and trait reward drive on food-related attentional bias. Both hunger and trait reward drive predicted degree of attentional "disengagement" from food images at short (100 ms), but not long (500, 2,000 ms) stimulus durations. Effects of hunger were found for both bland and appetizing foods, while effects of reward drive were restricted to appetizing foods. Our findings extend previous research showing delayed "disengagement" from threat-related stimuli, suggesting that both organismic- and goal-relevance are key biasing factors in attentional competition.
  • The results below are discovered through our pilot algorithms. Let us know how we are doing!

    • Bar-Haim, Y., Lamy, D., Pergamin, L., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M.J., & van Ijendoorn, M.H. (2007). Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and nonanxious individuals: A meta-analytic study. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 1-24.
    • Beaver, J.D., Lawrence, A.D., van Ditzhuijzen, J., Davis, M.H., Woods, A., & Calder, A.J. (2006). Individual differences in reward drive predict neural responses to images of food. The Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 5160-5166.
    • Brosch, T., Sander, D., & Scherer, K.R. (2007). That baby caught my eye…attention capture by infant faces. Emotion, 7, 685-689.
    • Brignell, C., Griffiths, T., Bradley, B.P., & Mogg, K. (2009). Attentional and approach biases for pictorial food cues. Influence of external eating. Appetite, 52, 299-306.
    • Bundesen, C. & Habekost, T. (2008). Principles of visual attention: linking mind and brain. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
    • Caseras, X., Avila, C., & Torrubia, R. (2003). The measurement of individual differences in behavioural inhibition and behavioural activation systems: a comparison of personality scales. Personality and Individual Differences, 34, 999- 1013.
    • Carver, C.S., & White, T.L. (1994). Behavioral inhibition, behavioural activation, and affective responses to impending reward and punishment: The BIS/BAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 319-333.
    • Castellanos, E.H., Charboneau, E., Dietrich, M.S., Park, S., Bradley, B.P., Mogg, K., & Cowan, R.L. (2009). Obese adults have visual attention bias for food cue images: evidence for altered reward system function. International Journal of Obesity, 33, 1063-1073.
    • Cox, W.M., Pothos, E.M., & Hosier, S.G. (2007). Cognitive-motivational predictors of excessive drinkers' success in changing. Psychopharmacology, 192, 499-510.
    • Davis, C., Patte, K., Levitan, R., Reid, C., Tweed, S., & Curtis, C. (2007). From motivation to behaviour: a model of reward sensitivity, overeating, and food preferences in the risk profile for obesity. Appetite, 48, 12-19.
    • Derryberry, D., & Reed, M.A. (2002). Anxiety-related attentional biases and their regulation by attentional control. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111, 225- 236.
    • Field, M., Mogg, K., & Bradley, B.P. (2004). Eye movements to smokingrelated cues: effects of nicotine deprivation. Psychopharmacology, 173, 116-123.
    • Fox, E., Russo, R., Bowles, R.J., & Dutton, K. (2001). Do threatening stimuli draw or hold visual attention in sub-clinical anxiety? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 681-700.
    • Franken, I.H.A., & Muris, P. (2005). Individual differences in reward sensitivity are related to food craving and relative body weight in healthy women. Appetite, 45, 198-201.
    • Georgiou, G.A., Bleakley, C., Hayward, J., Russo, R., Dutton, K., Eltiti, S., & Fox, E. (2005). Focusing on fear: attentional disengagement from emotional faces. Visual Cognition, 12, 145-158.
    • Hogarth, L., Dickinson, A., & Duka, T. (2009). Detection versus sustained attention to drug cues have dissociable roles in mediating drug seeking behaviour. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 17, 21-30.
    • Kemps, E., & Tiggemann, M. (2009). Attentional bias for craving-related (chocolate) food cues. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 17, 425-433.
    • Khayat, P.S., Spekreijse, H., & Roelfsema, P.R. (2006). Attention lights up new object representations before the old ones fade away. Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 138-142.
    • Koster, E.H.W., Crombez, G., Verschuere, B., & De Houwer, J. (2004). Selective attention to threat in the dot probe paradigm: differentiating vigilance and difficulty to disengage. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 42, 1183-1192.
    • Koster, E.H.W., Crombez, G., Verschuere, B., & De Houwer, J. (2006). Attention to threat in anxiety-prone individuals: mechanisms underlying attentional bias. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 30, 635-643.
    • LaBar, K.S., Gitelman, D.R., Parrish, T.B., Yun-Hee, K., Nobre, A.C., & Mesulam, M.M. (2001). Hunger selectively modulates corticolimbic activation to food stimuli in humans. Behavioral Neuroscience, 115, 493-500.
    • Lang, P.J., Bradley, M.M., & Cuthbert, B.N. (1997). Motivated attention: Affect, activation, and action. In P.J. Lang, R.F. Simons, & M.T. Balaban (Eds.), Attention and Orienting: Sensory and Motivational Processes (pp. 97-135). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
    • MacCallum, R. C., Zhang, S., Preacher, K. J., & Rucker, D. D. (2002). On the practice of dichotomization of quantitative variables. Psychological Methods, 7, 19- 40.
    • Mogg, K., Bradley, B.P., Hyare, H., & Lee, S. (1998). Selective attention to food-related stimuli in hunger: are attentional biases specific to emotional and psychopathological states, or are they also found in normal drive states? Behaviour Research and Therapy, 36, 227-237.
    • Maner, J.K., Gailliot, M.T., & DeWall, C.N. (2007). Adaptive attentional attunement: evidence for mating-related perceptual bias. Evolution and Human Behavior, 28, 28-36.
    • Mogg, K., Holmes, A., Garner, M., & Bradley, B.P. (2008). Effects of threat cues on attentional shifting, disengagement and response slowing in anxious individuals. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 46, 656-667.
    • Mohanty, A., Gitelman, D.R., Small, D.M., & Mesulam, M.M. (2008). The spatial attention network interacts with limbic and monoaminergic systems to modulate motivation-induced attention shifts. Cerebral Cortex, 18, 2604-2613.
    • Öhman, A. & Wiens, S (2004). The concept of an evolved fear module and cognitive theories of anxiety. In A.S.R. Manstead, N. Frijda, & A. Fischer (Eds.), Feelings and emotions: the Amsterdam symposium. (pp. 58-80). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
    • Osborne J.W., & Overbay, A. (2004). The power of outliers (and why researchers should ALWAYS check for them). Practical Assessment, Research & Evaluation, 9 (6). Retrieved from http://PAREonline.net/getvn.asp?v=9&n=6.
    • Papies E.K., Stroebe, W., & Aarts, H. (2008). The allure of forbidden food: on the role of attention in self-regulation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 1283-1292.
    • Placanica, J.L., Faunce, G.J., & Job, R.F. (2002). The effect of attentional biases for food and body shape/weight words in high and low eating disorder inventory scorers. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 32, 79-90.
    • Ratcliff, R. (1993). Methods for dealing with reaction time outliers. Psychological Bulletin, 114, 510-532.
    • Rodríguez, S., Fernández, M.C., Cepada-Benito, A., & Vila, J. (2005). Subjective and physiological reactivity to chocolate images in high and low chocolate cravers. Biological Psychology, 70, 9-18.
    • Salemink, E., van der Hout, M.A., & Kindt, M. (2007). Selective attention and threat: quick orienting versus slow disengagement and two versions of the dot probe task. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45, 607-615.
    • Sander, D., Grandjean, D., & Scherer, K. (2005). A systems approach to appraisal mechanisms in emotion. Neural Networks, 18, 317-352.
    • Stafford, L.D., & Yeomans, M.R. (2005). Caffeine deprivation state modulates coffee consumption but not attentional bias for caffeine-related stimuli. Behavioral Pharmacology, 16, 559-571.
    • Tapper, K., Pothos, E.M., Fadardi, J.S., & Ziori, E. (2008). Restraint, disinhibition and food-related processing bias. Appetite, 51, 335-338.
    • Van Strien, T., Frijters, J.E.R., Bergers, G.P.A., & Defares, P.B. (1986). The Dutch Eating Behavior Questionnaire (DEBQ) for assessment of restrained, emotional and external eating behaviour. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 5, 295-315.
    • Vuilleumier, P. (2005). How brains beware: neural mechanisms of emotional attention. Trends in cognitive sciences, 9, 585-594.
    • Vogt, J., De Houwer, J., Koster, E.H.W., Van Damme, S., & Crombez, G. (2008). Allocation of spatial attention to emotional stimuli depends on upon arousal and not valence. Emotion, 8, 880-885.
    • Yiend, J., & Mathews, A. (2001). Anxiety and attention to threatening pictures. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54A, 665-681.
  • No related research data.
  • No similar publications.

Share - Bookmark

Download from

Cite this article