LOGIN TO YOUR ACCOUNT

Username
Password
Remember Me
Or use your Academic/Social account:

CREATE AN ACCOUNT

Or use your Academic/Social account:

Congratulations!

You have just completed your registration at OpenAire.

Before you can login to the site, you will need to activate your account. An e-mail will be sent to you with the proper instructions.

Important!

Please note that this site is currently undergoing Beta testing.
Any new content you create is not guaranteed to be present to the final version of the site upon release.

Thank you for your patience,
OpenAire Dev Team.

Close This Message

CREATE AN ACCOUNT

Name:
Username:
Password:
Verify Password:
E-mail:
Verify E-mail:
*All Fields Are Required.
Please Verify You Are Human:
fbtwitterlinkedinvimeoflicker grey 14rssslideshare1
Bonin, Francesca; De Looze, Celine; Ghosh, Sucheta; Gilmartin, Emer; Vogel, Carl; Polychroniou, Anna; Salamin, Hugues; Vinciarelli, Alessandro; Campbell, Nick (2013)
Languages: English
Types: Other
Subjects: QA75
Conversational interaction is a dynamic activity in which participants engage in the construction of meaning and in establishing and maintaining social relationships. Lexical and prosodic accommodation have been observed in many studies as contributing importantly to these dimensions of social interaction. However, while previous works have considered accommodation mechanisms at global levels (for whole conversations, halves and thirds of conversations), this work investigates their evolution through repeated analysis at time intervals of increasing granularity to analyze the dynamics of alignment in a spoken language corpus. Results show that the levels of both prosodic and lexical accommodation fluctuate several times over the course of a conversation.
  • The results below are discovered through our pilot algorithms. Let us know how we are doing!

    • 1Trinity College Dublin, Dublin 2, Ireland 2 University of Glasgow, School of Computing Science, G128QQ Glasgow, UK
    • 3 Idiap Research Institute, CP592, 1920 Martigny, Switzerland [1] H. Giles, N. Coupland, and J. Coupland, “Accommodation the-
    • commodation: Developments in applied sociolinguistics, pp. 1-
    • 68, 1991. [2] J. Pardo, “On phonetic convergence during conversational interac-
    • tion,” The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, vol. 119,
    • p. 2382, 2006. [3] G. W. Stanford and S. Webster, “A Nonverbal Signal in Voices of
    • and Social Psychology, vol. 70, no. 6, pp. 1231-1240, 1996. [4] J. Edlund, M. Heldner, and J. Hirschberg, “Pause and gap length
    • ternational Speech Communication Association, 2009, pp. 2779-
    • 2782. [5] R. Levitan, A. Gravano, and J. Hirschberg, “Entrainment in
    • speech preceding backchannels,” Proc. of ACL 2011, 2011. [On-
    • line]. Available: http://www.aclweb.org/anthology/P/P11/P11-
    • 2020.pdf [6] C. De Looze, C. Oertel, S. Rauzy, and N. Campbell, “Measuring
    • speech,” in Proceedings of ICPhS. Springer, 2011. [7] S. Brennan, “Lexical entrainment in spontaneous dialog,” Pro-
    • ceedings of ISSD, pp. 41-44, 1996. [8] M. J. Pickering and S. Garrod, “Toward a mechanistic psychology
    • of dialogue.” The Behavioral and brain sciences, vol. 27, no. 2,
    • pp. 169-90; discussion 190-226, Apr. 2004. [Online]. Available:
    • http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15595235 [9] A. Nenkova, A. Gravano, and J. Hirschberg, “High frequency
    • word entrainment in spoken dialogue,” in Proceedings of the 46th
    • for Computational Linguistics, 2008, pp. 169-172. [10] H. Branigan, M. Pickering, J. Pearson, and J. McLean, “Linguistic
    • ics, vol. 42, no. 9, pp. 2355-2368, 2010. [11] M. Pickering and V. Ferreira, “Structural priming: a critical re-
    • view.” Psychological bulletin, vol. 134, no. 3, p. 427, 2008. [12] H. Giles, J. Coupland, and N. Coupland, Contexts of
    • Cambridge University Press, 1991. [Online]. Available:
    • http://books.google.ie/books?id=vnKCc4QO7gQC [13] P. Boylan, “Accommodation Theory Revisited,” University of
    • Rome III (Italy), Rome, Tech. Rep., 2004. [14] F. Parrill and I. Kimbara, “Seeing and hearing double: The influ-
    • Nonverbal Behavior, vol. 30, no. 4, pp. 157-166, 2006. [15] L. Tickle-Degnen and R. Rosenthal, “The Nature of Rapport and
    • Its Nonverbal Correlates,” Psychological inquiry, vol. 1, no. 4, pp.
    • 285-293, 2007. [16] J. L. Lakin and T. L. Chartrand, “Using nonconscious behavioral
    • cal Science, vol. 14, no. 4, pp. 334-339, 2003. [17] C. Shepard, H. Giles, and B. Le Poire, “Communication accom-
    • chology, no. 1.2, pp. 33-56, 2001. [18] L. Miles, L. Nind, and C. Macrae, “The rhythm of rapport: Inter-
    • tal social psychology, vol. 45, no. 3, pp. 585-589, 2009. [19] B. Vaughan, “Prosodic synchrony in co-operative task-based dia-
    • ings of Interspeech 2011. ISCA, 2011, pp. 1865-1868. [20] C. De Looze and S. Rauzy, “Measuring speakers' similarity in
    • Proceedings of Interspeech 2011. ISCA, 2011, pp. 1393-1396. [21] K. W. Church, “Empirical estimates of adaptation: the chance of
    • two noriegas is closer to p/2 than p 2,” in Proceedings of the 18th
    • conference on Computational linguistics-Volume 1. Association
    • for Computational Linguistics, 2000, pp. 180-186. [22] D. Reitter and J. Moore, “Predicting success in dialogue,” in Pro-
    • ceedings of the 45th Annual Meeting of the Association of Compu-
    • 2007, pp. 808-815. [23] A. Ward and D. Litman, “Automatically Measuring Lexical and
    • ISCA Tutorial and Research Workshop, 2007, p. 4. [24] F. Bonin, N. Campbell, and C. Vogel, “Laughter and topic
    • ceedings of CogInfoCom 2012 3rd IEEE International Confer-
    • ence on Cognitive Infocommunications, 2012, pp. 53-58. [25] R. Levitan and J. Hirschberg, “Measuring acoustic-prosodic en-
    • Association, 2011. [26] S. Kousidis, D. Dorran, C. McDonnell, and E. Coyle, “Times se-
    • in Proceedings of Interspeech, 2008. [28] C. De Looze, “Analyse et interpre´tation de l'empan temporel des
    • Ph.D. dissertation, Doctoral thesis, Universite´ de Provence, 2010. [29] H. Maganti, P. Motlicek, and D. Gatica-Perez, “Unsupervised
    • ence on Acoustics, Speech, and Signal Processing (ICASSP 2007),
    • vol. 4. IEEE, April 2007, pp. IV-1037-IV-1040. [30] N. H. De Jong and T. Wempe, “Praat script to detect syllable nu-
    • methods, vol. 41, no. 2, pp. 385-390, 2009. [31] F. Ramseyer and W. Tschacher, “Nonverbal synchrony or random
    • modal Interfaces: Active Listening and Synchrony, pp. 182-196,
    • 2010. [32] C. Vogel and L. Behan, “Measuring synchrony in dialog tran-
    • scripts,” in COST 2102 Training School, 2011, pp. 73-88. [33] C. Vogel, “Attribution of mutual understanding,” Journal of Law
    • & Policy, pp. 101-145, 2013. [34] T. Giannakopoulos, A. Pikrakis, and S. Theodoridis, “A novel
    • tion, 2008. ICPR 2008. 19th International Conference on. IEEE,
    • 2008, pp. 1-4. [35] P. N. Juslin and K. R. Scherer, Vocal expression of affect, ser.
    • Oxford University Press, 2005. [36] R. Banse and K. R. Scherer, “Acoustic profiles in vocal emo-
    • vol. 70, no. 3, pp. 614-636, 1996. [37] F. Goldman-Eisler, Psycholinguistics: Experiments in sponta-
    • neous speech. Academic Press, 1968. [38] F. Grosjean and A. Deschamps, “Analyse contrastive des variables
    • ables composantes, phe´nome`nes d'he´sitation,” Phonetica, vol. 31,
    • no. 3-4, pp. 144-184, 1975.
  • No related research data.
  • No similar publications.

Share - Bookmark

Download from

Cite this article