LOGIN TO YOUR ACCOUNT

Username
Password
Remember Me
Or use your Academic/Social account:

CREATE AN ACCOUNT

Or use your Academic/Social account:

Congratulations!

You have just completed your registration at OpenAire.

Before you can login to the site, you will need to activate your account. An e-mail will be sent to you with the proper instructions.

Important!

Please note that this site is currently undergoing Beta testing.
Any new content you create is not guaranteed to be present to the final version of the site upon release.

Thank you for your patience,
OpenAire Dev Team.

Close This Message

CREATE AN ACCOUNT

Name:
Username:
Password:
Verify Password:
E-mail:
Verify E-mail:
*All Fields Are Required.
Please Verify You Are Human:
fbtwitterlinkedinvimeoflicker grey 14rssslideshare1
Manches, A.D. (2010)
Languages: English
Types: Unknown
Subjects:
Despite their prevalence in early years’ education, there seems to be a lack of agreement over how or indeed whether physical objects support children's learning. Understanding the role of physically manipulating representations has gained impetus with the increasing potential to integrate digital technology into physical objects: tangible technology. This thesis aimed to evaluate the potential for tangible technologies to support numerical development by examining young children’s (4-8 years) use of physical objects in a numerical task. This task required them to find all the different ways in which a number (e.g., 7) can be decomposed (e.g., into 2 & 5). Seven carefully designed studies compared children’s numerical strategies using physical objects (cubes) with other materials (paper/virtual representations) or no materials. The studies showed that physical objects not only helped children identify solutions through simple physical actions, but fostered strategies that related solutions such as swapping groups of cubes or moving just one cube to get a new solution. This led to predictions about how a computer might influence strategies by constraining children’s actions to moving just one object at a time using the mouse. These predictions were confirmed, and a further study showed how using materials that changed colour according to the number grouped could support strategies by drawing children’s attention to numerical changes. The research showed that, to help children identify ways to break down a number efficiently, it was more effective to constrain their actions using a graphical, rather than tangible, interface. However, when multiple (physical) objects could be manipulated, children were able to constrain their own actions and used a wider range of strategies. Although moving multiple objects can be facilitated through interfaces such as tabletop computers, this research indicated certain cognitive benefits of physically manipulating representations for children’s numerical development that may inform tangible designs.
  • The results below are discovered through our pilot algorithms. Let us know how we are doing!

    • Abrahamson, D. (2004). Embodied Spatial Articulation: A Gesture Perspective on Student Negotiation between Kinesthetic Schemas and Epistemic Forms in Learning Mathematics. Paper presented at the Proceedings of the Twenty Sixth Annual Meeting of the North American Chapter of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education, Windsor, Ontario.
    • Abrahamson, D., & Howison, M. L. (2008). Kinematics: Kinetically induced mathematical learning [Electronic Version]. Presentation and Workshop at the UC Berkeley Gesture Study Group (Eve Sweetser, Director). Retrieved 6th June, 2009, from http://edrl.berkeley.edu/projects/kinemathics/MIT.mov.
    • Ackermann, E. (2001). Piaget's Constructivism, Papert's Constructionism: What's the difference. Future of learning group publication. MIT Media Lab, http://learning.media.mit.edu/content/publications/%20EA.Piaget%20_%20Pa pert.pdf.
    • Adams, J. W., & Hitch, G. J. (1997). Working memory and children's mental addition. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 67(1), 21-38.
    • Africano, D., Berg, S., Lindbergh, K., Lundholm, P., Nilbrink, F., & Persson, A. (2004). Designing tangible interfaces for children's collaboration. Paper presented at the CHI '04 extended abstracts on Human factors in computing systems.
    • Ainsworth, S., Wood, D., & O'Malley, C. (1998). There is more than one way to solve a problem: Evaluating a learning environment that supports the development of children's multiplication skills. Learning and Instruction, 8(2), 141-157.
    • Alibali, M. W., & DiRusso, A. A. (1999). The function of gesture in learning to count: More than keeping track. Cognitive Development, 14(1), 37-56.
    • Baroody, A. J., Wilkins, J. L. M., & Tiilikainen, S. H. (2003). The Development of Children's Understanding of Additive Commutativity: From Protoquantitative Concept to General Concept? In A. Baroody (Ed.), Development of Arithmetic Concepts and Skills: Constructing Adaptive Expertise (pp. 127-158). NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
    • Begel, A., Garcia, D., & Wolfman, S. (2004). Kinesthetic learning in the classroom. SIGCSE Bull., 36(1), 183-184.
    • Bisanz, J., Sherman, J., Rasmussen, C., & Ho, E. (2005). Development of Arithmetic Skills and Knowledge in Preschool Children. In J. Campbell (Ed.), Handbook of Mathematical Cognition (pp. 143-159). New York: Psychology Press.
    • Broaders, S. C., Cook, S. W., Mitchell, Z., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2007). Making children gesture brings out implicit knowledge and leads to learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology-General, 136(4), 539-550.
    • Bruner, J. (1966). Toward a theory of Instruction. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
    • Bryant, P., & Nunes, T. (1996). Children doing mathematics. London: Blackwell.
    • Burr, T. (2008). Mathematics Performance in Primary Schools: Getting the Best Results. London: National Audit Office/Department for Children, Schools and Families.
    • Canobi, K. H. (2005). Children's profiles of addition and subtraction understanding. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 92(3), 220-246.
    • Canobi, K. H. (2007). Commentary: Connecting Children's Informal Knowledge with School Mathematics. In V. Galwye (Ed.), Progress in Educational Psychology Research: New York: Nova Science Publishers.
    • Canobi, K. H., Reeve, R. A., & Pattison, P. E. (1998). The role of conceptual understanding in children's addition problem solving. Developmental Psychology, 34(5), 882-891.
    • Canobi, K. H., Reeve, R. A., & Pattison, P. E. (2002). Young Children's Understanding of Addition Concepts. Educational Psychology, 22(5), 513-532.
    • Canobi, K. H., Reeve, R. A., & Pattison, P. E. (2003). Patterns of knowledge in children's addition. Developmental Psychology, 39(3), 521-534.
    • Carey, S., & Spelke, E. S. (1994). Domain-specific knowledge and conceptual change. In L. Hirschfeld & S. Gelman (Eds.), Mapping the Mind (pp. 169-200). Cambridge, USA: Cambridge University Press.
    • Carlson, R. A., Avraamides, M. N., Cary, M., & Strasberg, S. (2007). What do the hands externalize in simple arithmetic? Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition, 33(4), 747-756.
    • Carpenter, T., Fennema, E., Fuson, K. C., Hiebert , J., Human, P. G., Hanlie, M., et al. (1999). Learning Basic Number Concepts and Skills as Problem Solving. In E. Fennema & T. Romberg (Eds.), Mathematics classrooms that promote understanding (pp. 45-61). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
    • Chao, S. J., Stigler, J. W., & Woodward, J. A. (2000). The effects of physical materials on kindergartners' learning of number concepts. Cognition and Instruction, 18(3), 285- 316.
    • Clark, A. (1999). An embodied cognitive science? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3(9), 345-351.
    • Clarke, B., Clarke, D., & Cheesman, J. (1996). The Mathematical Knowledge and Understanding Young Children Bring to School. Mathematics Education Research Journal, 18(1), 78-102.
    • Clements, D. H. (1999). ‗Concrete' Manipulatives, Concrete Ideas. Contemporary Issues in Early Childhood, 1(1), 45-60.
    • Clements, D. H. (2009). Arithmetic: Composition of Number, Place Value, and Multidigit Addition and Subtraction. In D. H. Clements (Ed.), Learning and Teaching Early Math: The Learning Trajectories Approach: Taylor & Francis.
    • Cobb, P., Yackel, E., & Wood, T. (1992). A Constructivist Alternative to the Representational View of Mind in Mathematics Education. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 23(1), 2-33.
  • No related research data.
  • No similar publications.

Share - Bookmark

Cite this article