LOGIN TO YOUR ACCOUNT

Username
Password
Remember Me
Or use your Academic/Social account:

CREATE AN ACCOUNT

Or use your Academic/Social account:

Congratulations!

You have just completed your registration at OpenAire.

Before you can login to the site, you will need to activate your account. An e-mail will be sent to you with the proper instructions.

Important!

Please note that this site is currently undergoing Beta testing.
Any new content you create is not guaranteed to be present to the final version of the site upon release.

Thank you for your patience,
OpenAire Dev Team.

Close This Message

CREATE AN ACCOUNT

Name:
Username:
Password:
Verify Password:
E-mail:
Verify E-mail:
*All Fields Are Required.
Please Verify You Are Human:
fbtwitterlinkedinvimeoflicker grey 14rssslideshare1
GRISON, Elise; GYSELINCK, Valérie; BURKHARDT, Jean Marie; WIENER, Jan Malte (2016)
Publisher: Springer Verlag
Languages: English
Types: Article
Subjects: VILLE, ITINERAIRE ROUTIER, PLANIFICATION, ZONE URBAINE, EYE-TRACKING, MOUVEMENT DE L'OEIL, MOBILITE
Planning routes using transportation network maps is a common task that has received little attention in the literature. Here, we present a novel eye-tracking paradigm to investigate psychological processes and mechanisms involved in such a route planning. In the experiment, participants were first presented with an origin and destination pair before we presented them with fictitious public transportation maps. Their task was to find the connecting route that required the minimum number of transfers. Based on participants' gaze behaviour, each trial was split into two phases: (1) the search for origin and destination phase, i.e., the initial phase of the trial until participants gazed at both origin and destination at least once and (2) the route planning and selection phase. Comparisons of other eye-tracking measures between these phases and the time to complete them, which depended on the complexity of the planning task, suggest that these two phases are indeed distinct and supported by different cognitive processes. For example, participants spent more time attending the centre of the map during the initial search phase, before directing their attention to connecting stations, where transitions between lines were possible. Our results provide novel insights into the psychological processes involved in route planning from maps. The findings are discussed in relation to the current theories of route planning.
  • The results below are discovered through our pilot algorithms. Let us know how we are doing!

    • Bailenson, J. N., Shum, M. S., & Uttal, D. H. (1998). Road climbing: Principles governing asymmetric route choices on maps. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 18(3), 251-264. doi:10.1006/jevp. 1998.0095.
    • Bailenson, J. N., Shum, M. S., & Uttal, D. H. (2000). The initial segment strategy: A heuristic for route selection. Memory and Cognition, 28(2), 306-318. doi:10.3758/BF03213808.
    • Beatty, J. (1982). Task-evoked pupillary responses, processing load, and the structure of processing resources. Psychological Bulletin, 91(2), 276-292.
    • Beck, M. R., Trenchard, M., van Lamsweerde, A., Goldstein, R. R., & Lohrenz, M. (2012). Searching in clutter: Visual attention strategies of expert pilots. Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting, 56, 1411-1415. doi:10.1177/1071181312561400.
    • Benshoof, J. A. (1970). Characteristics of drivers' route selection behavior. Traffic Engineering and Control, 11, 604-606.
    • Bovy, P. H. L., & Stern, E. (1990). Route choice: Wayfinding in transport networks. Transportation Research Part A: Policy and Practice, 27, 338-339. doi:10.1007/978-94-009-033-4.
    • Brunye´, T. T., Andonova, E., Meneghetti, C., Noordzij, M. L., Pazzaglia, F., Wienemann, R., … Taylor, H. A. (2012a). Planning routes around the world: International evidence for southern route preferences. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 32(4), 297-304. doi:10.1016/j.jenvp.2012.05.003.
    • Brunye´, T. T., Gagnon, S. A., Waller, D., Hodgson, E., TowerRichardi, S., & Taylor, H. A. (2012b). Up north and down south: Implicit associations between topography and cardinal direction. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65(10), 1880-1894. doi:10.1080/17470218.2012.663393.
    • Brunye´, T. T., Mahoney, C. R., Gardony, A. L., & Taylor, H. A. (2010). North is up(hill): Route planning heuristics in real-world environments. Memory and Cognition, 38(6), 700-712. doi:10. 3758/MC.38.6.700.
    • Brunye´, T. T., & Taylor, H. A. (2009). When goals constrain: Eye movements and memory for goal-oriented map study. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 23(6), 772-787. doi:10.1002/acp.1508.
    • Casakin, H., Barkowsky, T., Klippel, A., & Freksa, C. (2000). Schematic maps as way finding aids. In C. Freksa, C. Habel, & K. F. Wender (Eds.), Spatial cognition II: An interdisciplinary approach to representing and processing spatial knowledge (pp. 54-71). Berlin: Springer.
    • Cazzato, V., Basso, D., Cutini, S., & Bisiacchi, P. (2010). Gender differences in visuospatial planning: An eye movements study. Behavioural Brain Research, 206(2), 177-183. doi:10.1016/j. bbr.2009.09.010.
    • Chowdhury, S., & Ceder, A. (2013). The effect of interchange attributes on public-transport users' intention to use routes involving transfers. Psychology and Behavioral Sciences, 2(1), 5-13. doi:10.11648/j.pbs.20130201.12.
    • Conroy Dalton, R. (2003). The secret is to follow your nose: Route path selection and angularity. Environment and Behavior, 35(1), 107-131. doi:10.1177/0013916502238867.
    • de Condappa, O., & Wiener, J. (2016). Human place and response learning: Navigation strategy selection, pupil size and gaze behavior. Psychological Research, 80(1), 82-93.
    • Ettema, D., Friman, M., Ga¨rling, T., Olsson, L. E., & Fujii, S. (2012). How in-vehicle activities affect work commuters' satisfaction with public transport. Journal of Transport Geography, 24, 215-222. doi:10.1016/j.jtrangeo.2012.02.007.
    • Farr, A. C., Kleinschmidt, T., Yarlagadda, P., & Mengersen, K. (2012). Wayfinding: A simple concept, a complex process. Transport Reviews, 32(6), 715-743. doi:10.1080/01441647. 2012.712555.
    • Freksa, C. (1999). Spatial aspects of task-specific wayfinding maps: A representation-theoretic perspective. In J. S. Gero & B. Tversky (Eds.), Visual and spatial reasoning in design (pp. 15-32). Sydney: Key Centre of Design Computing and Cognition, University of Sydney.
    • Friman, M. (2010). Affective dimensions of the waiting experience. Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour, 13(3), 197-205. doi:10.1016/j.trf.2010.04.006.
    • Garland, H. C., Haynes, J. J., & Grubb, G. C. (1979). Transit map color coding and street detail. Environment and Behavior, 11(2), 162-184. doi:10.1177/0013916579112002.
    • Ga¨rling, T., & Ga¨rling, E. (1988). Distance minimization in downtown pedestrian shopping. Environment and Planning A, 20, 547-554. doi:10.1068/a200547.
    • Ga¨rling, T., Lindberg, E., & Ma¨ntyla¨, T. (1983). Orientation in buildings: Effects of familiarity, visual access, and orientation aids. Journal of Applied Psychology, 68, 177-186. doi:10.1037/ 00219010.68.1.177.
    • Ga¨rling, T., Sa¨isa¨, J., Book, A., & Lindberg, E. (1986). The spatiotemporal sequencing of everyday activities in the largescale environment. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 6(4), 261-280. doi:10.1016/S0272-4944(86)80001-9.
    • Golledge, R. (1995). Path selection and route preference in human navigation: A progress report. In A. U. Frank & W. Kuhn (Eds.), Spatial information theory: A theoretical basis for GIS (COSIT'95). Lecture notes in computer science, no. 988 (pp. 207-222). Berlin: Springer.
    • Golledge, R. G. (1999). Human cognitive maps and wayfinding. In R. G. Golledge (Ed.), Wayfinding behavior: Cognitive mapping and other spatial processes (pp. 1-45). Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.
    • Guo, Z. (2011). Mind the map! The impact of transit maps on path choice in public transit. Transportation Research Part A: Policy and Practice, 45(7), 625-639. doi:10.1016/j.tra.2011.04.001.
    • Hayes-Roth, B., & Hayes-Roth, F. (1979). A cognitive model of planning. Cognitive Science, 3(4), 275-310. doi:10.1207/ s15516709cog0304_1.
    • Henderson, J. M., Weeks, P. A, Jr., & Hollingworth, A. (1999). The effects of semantic consistency on eye movements during complex scene viewing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25(1), 210. doi:10.1037/ 00961523.25.1.210.
    • Hine, J., & Scott, J. (2000). Seamless, accessible travel: Users' views of the public transport journey and interchange. Transport Policy, 7(3), 217-226. doi:10.1016/S0967-070X(00)00022-6.
    • Ho¨lscher, C., Tenbrink, T., & Wiener, J. M. (2011). Would you follow your own route description? Cognitive strategies in urban route planning. Cognition, 121(2), 228-247. doi:10.1016/j. cognition.2011.06.005.
    • Just, M. A., & Carpenter, P. A. (1980). A theory of reading: From eye fixations to comprehension. Psychological Review, 87(4), 329-354. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.87.4.829.
    • MacGregor, J. N., Chronicle, E. P., & Ormerod, T. C. (2004). Convex hull or crossing avoidance? Solution heuristics in the traveling salesperson problem. Memory and Cognition, 32(2), 260-270. doi:10.3758/BF03196857.
    • MacGregor, J. N., & Chu, Y. (2011). Human performance on the traveling salesman and related problems: A review. The Journal of Problem Solving, 3(2), 1-29. doi:10.7771/1932-6246.1090.
    • MacGregor, J. N., & Ormerod, T. (1996). Human performance on the traveling salesman problem. Perception and Psychophysics, 58(4), 527-539. doi:10.3758/BF03213088.
    • MacGregor, J. N., Ormerod, T. C., & Chronicle, E. P. (1999). Spatial and contextual factors in human performance on the travelling salesperson problem. Perception, 28(11), 1417-1427. doi:10. 1068/p2863.
    • Meilinger, T., Ho¨lscher, C., Bu¨chner, S. J., & Bro¨ samle, M. (2007). How much information do you need? Schematic maps in wayfinding and self localisation. In T. Barkowsky (Ed.), Spatial cognition V-reasoning, action, interaction (pp. 381-400). Berlin: Springer. doi:10.1007/978-3-540-75666-8_22.
    • Neider, M. B., & Zelinsky, G. J. (2006). Scene context guides eye movements during visual search. Vision Research, 46(5), 614-621. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2005.08.025.
    • Pomplun, M., Reingold, E. M., & Shen, J. (2001). Investigating the visual span in comparative search: The effects of task difficulty and divided attention. Cognition, 81(2), B57-B67. doi:10.1016/ S0010-0277(01)00123-8.
    • Raveau, S., Guo, Z., Mun˜oz, J. C., & Wilson, N. H. M. (2014). A behavioural comparison of route choice on metro networks: Time, transfers, crowding, topology and socio-demographics. Transportation Research Part A: Policy and Practice, 66, 185-195. doi:10.1016/j.tra.2014.05.010.
    • Roberts, M. J. (2008). Underground maps after Beck (2nd ed.). Harrow Weald: Capital Transport Publishing.
    • Roberts, M. J., Newton, E. J., Lagattolla, F. D., Hughes, S., & Hasler, M. C. (2013). Objective versus subjective measures of Paris Metro map usability: Investigating traditional octolinear versus all-curves schematics. International Journal of Human Computer Studies, 71, 363-386. doi:10.1016/j.ijhcs.2012.09.004.
    • Sobel, K. V., Gerrie, M. P., Poole, B. J., & Kane, M. J. (2007). Individual differences in working memory capacity and visual search: The roles of top-down and bottom-up processing. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 14(5), 840-845. doi:10. 3758/BF03194109.
    • Van Orden, K. F., Limbert, W., Makeig, S., & Jung, T. P. (2001). Eye activity correlates of workload during a visuospatial memory task. Human Factors: The Journal of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, 43(1), 111-121.
    • Vickers, D., Lee, M. D., Dry, M., & Hughes, P. (2003). The roles of the convex hull and the number of potential intersections in performance on visually presented traveling salesperson problems. Memory and Cognition, 31(7), 1094-1104. doi:10.3758/ BF03196130.
    • Wiener, J. M., Ehbauer, N. N., & Mallot, H. A. (2009). Planning paths to multiple targets: Memory involvement and planning heuristics in spatial problem solving. Psychological Research, 73(5), 644-658. doi:10.1007/s00426-008-0181-3.
    • Wiener, J. M., & Tenbrink, T. (2008). Traveling salesman problem: The human case. Ku¨nstliche Intelligenz. KI und Kognition, 1(08), 18-22.
    • Wolfe, J. M. (1994). Guided Search 2.0 a revised model of visual search. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 1(2), 202-238. doi:10.3758/BF03200774.
  • No related research data.
  • No similar publications.

Share - Bookmark

Cite this article