LOGIN TO YOUR ACCOUNT

Username
Password
Remember Me
Or use your Academic/Social account:

CREATE AN ACCOUNT

Or use your Academic/Social account:

Congratulations!

You have just completed your registration at OpenAire.

Before you can login to the site, you will need to activate your account. An e-mail will be sent to you with the proper instructions.

Important!

Please note that this site is currently undergoing Beta testing.
Any new content you create is not guaranteed to be present to the final version of the site upon release.

Thank you for your patience,
OpenAire Dev Team.

Close This Message

CREATE AN ACCOUNT

Name:
Username:
Password:
Verify Password:
E-mail:
Verify E-mail:
*All Fields Are Required.
Please Verify You Are Human:
fbtwitterlinkedinvimeoflicker grey 14rssslideshare1
Botting, N.; Conti-Ramsden, G. (2008)
Publisher: British Psychological Society
Languages: English
Types: Article
Subjects: P1, HN

Classified by OpenAIRE into

mesheuropmc: behavioral disciplines and activities
Social skill and language are known to relate, not least in the example of those with specific language impairment (SLI). However, most of the research examining this trend has been conducted on young primary school age children and the nature of the relationships is unclear. Furthermore, little is known about which young people in general have social difficulties and whether language, social cognition, and social skills are directly associated at this age. In this study, a large cohort made up of young people with a history of SLI (N = 134) and a typically developing (TD) group (N = 124) of the same age were followed up in their final year of compulsory schooling (aged 16). Language, social cognition, social skills, and functional social outcomes (friendships and levels of social activity) were assessed using tasks and questionnaires. Modest associations were found between social cognition, language, and social behaviours, the strongest being between language and social cognition. Regression analyses showed that as a combined group, the adolescents' functional social outcomes were most associated with expressive language, social skill, and social cognitive ability. However, the patterns differed when the groups were analysed separately, with social cognition playing more of a role for those with SLI. These findings suggest that poor language may play a complex role in adolescents' social development.
  • The results below are discovered through our pilot algorithms. Let us know how we are doing!

    • Baron-Cohen, S. (1985) Mindblindness: An essay on autism and theory of mind. Cambridge MA: MIT Press.
    • Baron-Cohen, S. Wheelwright, S., Hill, J., Raste, Y., & Plumb, I. (2001). The 'Reading the Mind in the eyes' test revised version: A study with normal adults, and adults with Asperger Syndrome or High-Functioning autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42, 241-252
    • Bavin, E. L., Wilson, P., Maruff, P., & Sleeman, F. (2005). Spatio-visual memory of children with specific language impairment: Evidence for generalised
    • Coleman, J. C. (1980). The nature of adolescence. London: Methuen.
    • Conti-Ramsden, G., & Botting, N. (1999a). Classification of children with specific language impairment: Longitudinal considerations. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 42, 1195-1204.
    • Conti-Ramsden, G., & Botting, N. (1999b). Characteristics of children attending language units in England: A national study of 7-year-olds. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 34, 359-366.
    • Conti-Ramsden, G., & Botting, N. (2004). Social difficulties and victimization in children with SLI at 11 years of age. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 47, 145-161.
    • Conti-Ramsden, G., Botting, N., & Faragher, B. (2001). Psycholinguistic markers for specific language impairment (SLI). Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42, 741-748.
    • Conti-Ramsden, G., Crutchley, A., & Botting, N. (1997). The extent to which psychometric tests differentiate subgroups of children with SLI. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 40, 765-777.
    • Conti-Ramsden, G., Simkin, Z., & Botting, N. (2006). The prevalence of ASD in adolescents with a history of SLI. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, 621-628.
    • de Villiers, J. G., & de Villiers, P. A. (2000). Linguistic determinism and the understanding of false beliefs. In P. Mitchell and K. J. Riggs (Eds.) Children's reasoning and the mind. (pp. 191-228). Hove, England: Psychology Press/Taylor & Francis (UK).
    • Farmer, M. (2000). Language and Social Cognition in Children with Specific Language Impairment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,41(5), 627-636.
    • Fujiki, M., Brinton, B., Isaacson, T. & Summers, C. (2001). Social behaviors of children with language impairment on the playground: A pilot study. Language, Speech and Hearing Services in Schools, 32(2), 101-113.
    • Gathercole, S.E., & Baddeley, A.D. (1990). Phonological memory deficits in language disordered children: Is there a causal connection? Journal of Memory and Language, 29, 336-360.
    • Gillon, G. & Dodd, B. J. (2005). A prospective study of the relationship between phonological, semantic and syntactic skills and specific reading disability. Reading and Writing, 6, 321-345.
    • Goodman, R. (1997). The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38, 581-586.
    • Hale, M., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2003). The influence of language on theory of 24(2), 129-154.
    • Harris, P. L. (2005). Conversation, pretence and theory of mind. In P. Carruthers and P. K. Smith (Eds.) Why language matters for theory of mind (p.200-220). Cambridge UK; Cambridge University Press.
    • Heavey, L., Phillips, W., Baron-Cohen, S., & Rutter, M. (2000). The awkward moments test: A naturalistic measure of social understanding in autism Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30(3), 225-236.
    • Herba, C. & Phillips, M. (2004). Annotation: Development of facial expression recognition from childhood to adolescence: behavioural and neurological perspectives. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 1185-1198.
    • Hick, R., Botting, N., & Conti-Ramsden, G. (2005). Cognitive abilities in children with specific language impairment (SLI): Consideration of visuo-spatial skills. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 40(2), 137- 149.
    • Howlin, P., Mawhood, L., & Rutter, M. (2000). Autism and developmental receptive language disorder - a follow-up comparison in early adult life. II: Social, behavioural, and psychiatric outcomes. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41, 561-578.
    • Leonard, L. (2003). Specific language impairment: Characterizing the deficit. In Y. Levy, & J. Schaeffer (Eds.), Towards a definition of SLI (pp. 209-231). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
    • Lohmann, H. & Tomasello, M. (2003). The role of language in the development of false belief understanding: A training study. Child Development, 74(4), 1145-1157.
    • Maestro, S., Muratori, F., Cavallaro, M. C., Pei, F., Stern, D., Golse, B., & PalacioEspasa, F. (2002). Attentional skills during the first 6 months of age in autism spectrum disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 41(10), 1239-1245.
    • Miller, C.A. (2004). False belief and sentence complement performance in children with specific language impairment. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 39(2), 191-213.
    • Norbury, C. F. (2005). The relationship between theory of mind and metaphor: Evidence from children with language impairment and autistic spectrum disorder. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 23(3), 383-399.
    • Pratt, C., Botting, N., & Conti-Ramsden, G. (2006). Characteristics and concerns of mothers of adolescents with SLI. Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 22 (2), 177-196.
    • Raven, J. C. (1986). Coloured Progressive Matrices. London: H. K. Lewis & Co.
    • Redmond, S., & Rice, M. (1998). The socioemotional behaviours of children with SLI: Social adaptation or social deviance? Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 41, 688-700.
    • Renfrew, C. (1991). The bus story: A test of continuous speech. C. E. Renfrew: Oxford, UK.
    • Rutter, M. (1967). A children's behaviour questionnaire for completion by teachers: Preliminary findings. Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 8, 1-11.
    • Rutter, M., Graham, P., Chadwick, O., & Yule, W. (1976). Adolescent turmoil: fact or fiction? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 17, 35-56.
    • Semel, E., Wiig, E., & Secord, W. (1987). Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals - Revised. San Antonio, TX : The Psychological Corporation.
    • Shields, J., Varley, R., Broks, P., & Simpson, A. (1996). Social cognition in developmental language disorders and high level autism. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 38, 487-495.
  • No related research data.
  • No similar publications.

Share - Bookmark

Funded by projects

  • WT

Cite this article